Handling Stress Does Not Reflect Personality in Female Zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata)

Similar documents
Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

How Does Photostimulation Age Alter the Interaction Between Body Size and a Bonus Feeding Program During Sexual Maturation?

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

Relationship between hatchling length and weight on later productive performance in broilers

An Experimental Test of Condition-Dependent Male and Female Mate Choice in Zebra Finches

Song in the city: the effects of urban noise on communication patterns and population genetics of an Australian passerine

Evolution in Action: Graphing and Statistics

How do low-quality females know they re low-quality and do they always prefer low-quality mates?

Gray, G. (2016) Enhancing the welfare of zebra finches through the use of environmental enrichment. Animal Technology and Welfare, 15(3), pp

Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) are breeding earlier at Creamer s Field Migratory Waterfowl Refuge, Fairbanks, AK

Biol 160: Lab 7. Modeling Evolution

769 q 2005 The Royal Society

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc

The effect of testosterone injections on aggression and begging behaviour of black headed gull chicks (Larus ridibundus)

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns

Egg size, offspring sex and hatching asynchrony in zebra finches Taeniopygia guttata

Supporting Online Material for

Reproductive success and symmetry in zebra finches

Back to basics - Accommodating birds in the laboratory setting

Animal Behaviour. The role of adult experience in nest building in the zebra finch, Taeniopygia guttata. Felicity Muth, Susan D.

doi: /

Lab 7. Evolution Lab. Name: General Introduction:

Building Rapid Interventions to reduce antimicrobial resistance and overprescribing of antibiotics (BRIT)

Egg laying in the Blue Tit (Parus caeruleus):

Maternal investment during egg laying and offspring sex: an experimental study of zebra finches

The effects of environmental and individual quality on reproductive performance Amininasab, Seyed Mehdi

Selection for Egg Mass in the Domestic Fowl. 1. Response to Selection

Unit 3 Sustainability and interdependence Sub Topic 3.4: Animal welfare

Parental food provisioning is related to nestling stress response in wild great tit nestlings: implications for the development of personality

Vitellogenin dynamics during egg-laying: daily variation, repeatability and relationship with egg size

Food preference and copying behaviour in zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata

EFFECTS OF ENVIRONMENTAL TEMPERATURE, RELATIVE HUMIDITY, FASTING AND FEEDING ON THE BODY TEMPERATURE OF LAYING HENS

From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research. Animal behavior then & now

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories

Correlation of. Animal Science Biology & Technology, 3/E, by Dr. Robert Mikesell/ MeeCee Baker, 2011, ISBN 10: ; ISBN 13:

Chapter 16: Evolution Lizard Evolution Virtual Lab Honors Biology. Name: Block: Introduction

Genetic variation in cue sensitivity involved in avian timing of reproduction

An experimental test of female choice relative to male structural coloration in eastern bluebirds

Applied Animal Behaviour Science 126 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Applied Animal Behaviour Science journal homepage:

Factors Influencing Local Recruitment in Tree Swallows, Tachycineta bicolor

Anas clypeata (Northern Shoveler)

EVALUATION OF EFFECTS OF A STRAIN, STOCKING DENSITY AND AGE ON BILATERAL SYMMETRY OF BROILER CHICKENS

Mendelian Genetics Using Drosophila melanogaster Biology 12, Investigation 1

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories

BEHAVIOURAL DIFFERENCES IN WOLVES AND DOGS. Christina Hansen Wheat

Overview LANCTB1. Observe, assess and respond to the behaviour of dogs. Observe, assess and respond to the behaviour of dogs

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories

[ Post a Response Precious Fids Chat ] Novel Chemistry at Work To Provide Parrot's Vibrant Red Colors.

Research Thesis. by Nathaniel J. Sackinger. The Ohio State University June 2013

Effects of Cage Stocking Density on Feeding Behaviors of Group-Housed Laying Hens

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

Evaluating the quality of evidence from a network meta-analysis

Husbandry Guidelines Name Species Prepared by

SEXUAL SELECTION ON PLUMAGE COLOR IN A NORTH CAROLINA POPULATION OF EASTERN BLUEBIRDS. Callie Lynn Younginer. Honors Thesis

Effects of a Pre-Molt Calcium and Low-Energy Molt Program on Laying Hen Behavior During and Post-Molt

EFFECTS OF COMMON ORIGIN AND COMMON ENVIRONMENT ON NESTLING PLUMAGE COLORATION IN THE GREAT TIT (PARUS MAJOR)

Procnias averano (Bearded Bellbird)

Gulf and Caribbean Research

The Use of Cage Enrichment to Reduce Male Mouse Aggression Neil Ambrose & David B. Morton Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

New York State Cooperative Wildlife Health Program

A-l. Students shall examine the circulatory and respiratory systems of animals.

Everyday Mysteries: Why most male birds are more colorful than females

SEXUAL IMPRINTING IN FEMALE ZEBRA FINCHES: CHANGES IN PREFERENCES AS AN EFFECT OF ADULT EXPERIENCE

Comparative Evaluation of Online and Paper & Pencil Forms for the Iowa Assessments ITP Research Series

NATURAL AND SEXUAL VARIATION

THE BEGGING BEHAVIOR OF NESTLING EASTERN SCREECH-OWLS

Correlates and Consequences of the Pair Bond in Steller s Jays

Do Tachycineta swallows use public information to choose nest sites?

Pilot study to identify risk factors for coprophagic behaviour in dogs

These small issues are easily addressed by small changes in wording, and should in no way delay publication of this first- rate paper.

Welcome to Darwin Day!

The welfare of laying hens

Does begging affect growth in nestling tree swallows, Tachycineta bicolor?

Biology. Slide 1 of 33. End Show. Copyright Pearson Prentice Hall

Evolution of Birds. Summary:

Structural and melanin coloration indicate parental effort and reproductive success in male eastern bluebirds

Effects of Natural Selection

Variation in Piglet Weights: Development of Within-Litter Variation Over a 5-Week Lactation and Effect of Farrowing Crate Design

Influence of adult courtship experience on the development of sexual preferences in zebra finch males*

Male parental care and monogamy in snow buntings

Key words : rabbit synthetic line local population reproduction - adaptation hot climate. Introduction

Genetic analysis of mentality traits in Rhodesian Ridgeback dogs

Primate Welfare Meeting

Avian Ecology: Life History, Breeding Seasons, & Territories

COOPERATIVE EXTENSION Bringing the University to You

clutch size and escape take-off speed in female zebra finches

Mr. Bouchard Summer Assignment AP Biology. Name: Block: Score: / 20. Topic: Chemistry Review and Evolution Intro Packet Due: 9/4/18

A future cost of misdirected parental care for brood parasitic young?

MANAGEMENT GUIDELINES PARENT STOCK

Visual aids to increase the awareness of condition scoring of sheep - a model approach

rodent species in Australia to the fecal odor of various predators. Rattus fuscipes (bush

08 alberts part2 7/23/03 9:10 AM Page 95 PART TWO. Behavior and Ecology

Key considerations in the breeding of macaques and marmosets for scientific purposes

Activity 1: Changes in beak size populations in low precipitation

Co-operative breeding by Long-tailed Tits

CLUSTERING AND GENETIC ANALYSIS OF BODY RESERVES CHANGES THROUGHOUT PRODUCTIVE CYCLES IN MEAT SHEEP

Cover picture with friendly permission extracted from a photo by: Barry O`Malley, Sydney, Australia (

Mental stim ulation it s not just for dogs!! By Danielle Middleton- Beck BSc hons, PGDip CABC

5 State of the Turtles

Genetics of temperament: What do we know about the back test?

Transcription:

Journal of Comparative Psychology 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol., No., 000 000 0735-7036/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0024636 Handling Stress Does Not Reflect Personality in Female Zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata) Morgan David Université de Bourgogne and Université du Québec à Montréal Yannick Auclair and François-Xavier Dechaume-Moncharmont Université de Bourgogne Frank Cézilly Université de Bourgogne and Institut Universitaire de France Although increasing attention is given to both the causes and consequences of variation in animal personality, the measurement of personality in captive or free-ranging individuals remains an issue. In particular, one important question concerns whether personality should be established from the existence of complex behavioral syndromes (a suite of correlated behavioral traits) or could be more easily deduced from a single variable. In that context, it has recently been suggested that handling stress, measured through breathing rate during handling, could be a good descriptor of personality, at least in passerine birds. The authors experimentally investigated to what extent handling stress was correlated with personality in female zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata), as assessed from a suite of repeatable behavioral traits, including activity, exploratory behavior, neophobia, and reaction to startle. Although breathing rate was repeatable across individuals, it was not related to any behavioral trait, suggesting that it cannot be used to quickly predict personality, at least in zebra finches. Breathing rate during handling, in addition, was related to morphology, questioning the fact that breathing rate during handling reflects personality irrespective of individual state. The authors suggest that inference on global personality from a reduced number of traits should be performed with caution. Keywords: behavioral syndrome, temperament, breathing rate, exploration, bootstrap Interindividual differences in behavior (so-called personalities ) have recently become of prime interest to ornithologists and behavioral ecologists (Réale, Reader, Sol, McDougall, & Dingemanse, 2007). Indeed, recent evidence has shown that personality Morgan David, Équipe Écologie Évolutive, UMR CNRS 5561 Biogéosciences, Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France, and Groupe de Recherche en Écologie Comportementale et Animale, Département des Sciences Biologiques, Université du Québec à Montréal, Montréal, Québec, Canada; Yannick Auclair and François-Xavier Dechaume-Moncharmont, Équipe Écologie Évolutive, UMR CNRS 5561 Biogéosciences, Université de Bourgogne; Frank Cézilly, Équipe Écologie Évolutive, UMR CNRS 5561 Biogéosciences, Université de Bourgogne, and Institut Universitaire de France. Yannick Auclair is now at the Animal Behaviour Group, Institute of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zürich- Irchel, Zürich Switzerland. We thank Vincent Careau for his advice on bird metabolism, Sébastien Motreuil for assistance during bird care and the building of experimental devices, and Elise Melloul for assistance during the experiments. We thank three anonymous referees for their valuable comments, which helped to improve the article. This work was funded with a PhD grant to Morgan David and a master honorific grant to Yannick Auclair, both provided by the Conseil Régional de Bourgogne, Dijon, France. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Morgan David, Université de Bourgogne, Équipe Écologie Évolutive, UMR CNRS 5561 Biogéosciences, 6 boulevard Gabriel, 21000 Dijon, France. E-mail: morgan.david@u-bourgogne.fr traits, such as neophobic or exploratory tendencies, can have fitness consequences (Smith & Blumstein, 2008) and can evolve through natural (Quinn, Patrick, Bouwhuis, Wilkin, & Sheldon, 2009) and sexual selection (Schuett, Tregenza, & Dall, 2010). It is interesting to note that some personality traits can be found to be related to each other at the population level, thus, forming a behavioral syndrome (Wilson et al., 2010). These correlations have several implications for behavioral studies. First, they suggest that different behaviors do not evolve independently but act as constraints toward each other, setting the stage for an integrative study of behavior (Sih, Bell, & Johnson, 2004). Second, these correlations, once established, can potentially enable experimenters to assess only a reduced number of traits to determine individual global personality (Fucikova, Drent, Smits, & van Oers, 2009). For instance, in great tits (Parus major), breathing rate after handling has been related to both exploratory tendencies and neophobia and used as an indicator of individual personality (Carere & van Oers, 2004; Fucikova et al., 2009). Exploratory birds had a higher breathing rate during handling (Fucikova et al., 2009), whereas bold individuals had a lower breathing rate (Carere & van Oers, 2004). These results suggest that breathing rate after handling might be used a proxy for personality in other bird species, given its ease of measurement in both field and lab conditions. Recent evidence, however, suggests that the assessment of personality may not always be reduced to the measurement of one single behavioral trait. For instance, David, Auclair, and Cézilly (2011) found that although neophobia, exploratory tendencies, activity, 1

2 DAVID, AUCLAIR, DECHAUME-MONCHARMONT, AND CÉZILLY and reaction to a startle were related to each other within a broad behavioral syndrome in zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata), obstinacy, while showing very high repeatability (i.e., withinindividual consistency through time), was not related to any other personality trait (David et al., 2011). The use of a quickly assessable trait, such as breathing rate after handling, as a general proxy for personality in bird species then deserves further consideration. Therefore, following Fucikova et al. (2009), we investigated to what extent breathing rate that follows handling can predict personality in the zebra finch, while controlling for the effect of body mass and body condition on handling stress. Method Study Subjects Forty-one wild-type female zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata), in which personality had been assessed in a previous study (David et al., 2011), were kept in home cages (60 30 33 cm), by unisex groups of two or three birds, before the experiments. Zebra finches are small monogamous and gregarious passerine birds, widely used in behavior studies (Zann, 1996). Each individual was identified with an orange numbered ring (AC Hughes, Hampton Hill, England; size XF). Only females were used, as this study was part of a larger research program on the influence of personality on female sexual behavior. Our captive stock was kept in a breeding room where temperature was maintained at 22 2 C, and the photoperiod was 13:11-hr light dark cycle (7:30 a.m. 8:30 p.m.). Birds were provisioned with millet seeds, cuttlebones, and water ad libitum. At the end of the experiment, birds were kept in groups of two to be used in subsequent studies. The experimental procedures were in agreement with the ethical requirements of the Université de Bourgogne, France. Assessment of Personality Traits Birds were food deprived for one hour before each trial to control for hunger levels. All tests were performed on individuals kept single. Activity corresponded to the number of movements performed in the home cage during a 10-min period. Neophobia was assessed from the tendency to feed near and to approach a novel object in the home cage. Exploratory tendencies were measured from the number of movements performed in a large unfamiliar cage (140 140 cm and 70 cm high) containing five artificial trees, each composed of four small branches. Reaction to a startle corresponded to the latency time to resume foraging in the home cage after a stressful event. All these personality traits have been found to be highly repeatable in zebra finches in a previous study (David et al., 2011). Detailed information concerning experimental apparatus and methodology can be found in David et al. (2011). Handling Stress Following previous studies conducted on small passerines (Carere & van Oers, 2004; Fucikova et al., 2009), we assessed handling stress from the intensity of breathing rate after being caught by an experimenter. All birds were kept single in their home cage. One experimenter (Y.A.) caught each bird in its own home cage and measured the number of breast movements during 20 s as a measure of breathing rate (see Carere, Welink, Drent, Koolhaas, & Groothuis, 2001) while handling the bird on the back. Catching latency was also quantified as the latency time to catch the bird within its home cage. This procedure was repeated twice with a one-week interval between both trials, and both measures were averaged for each individual. All trials were performed at the same time of the day (6:00 p.m.) for all individuals in order to discard the potential effect of diurnal variation in behavioral activity or stress response. Handling-stress trials were conducted one day before personality tests began. Morphometric Measures All morphometric measures were taken twice on two different days by Y.A and two days before starting personality tests. Tarsus length was measured with a digital caliper to the nearest 0.1 mm. Birds were weighed to the nearest 0.01 g with an electronic balance (Ohaus, Scout Pro SPU202) at the same time of day for each individual. Values were then averaged for each bird. We determined body condition through the residuals calculated from the linear regression of log 10 (body mass) on log 10 (tarsus length; Schamber, Esler, & Flint, 2009). Statistical Analyses Consistency (R) of catching latency and breathing rate were assessed through Lessells and Boag s (1987) method. Differences between both handling trials were assessed through paired t tests, followed by the calculation of Cohen s d as a measure of effect size (Nakagawa & Cuthill, 2007). Statistics are reported with mean or R and 95% confidence interval (CI; Nakagawa & Schielzeth, 2010). Relationships between personality and breathing rate were tested by using Pearson s correlations. All statistical analyses were performed with JMP 5.0.1 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). Results Overall, all measured behaviors were repeatable. However, whereas breathing rate was found to be related to body mass or catching latency, no relationship was found between breathing rate and any of the personality traits: Mean catching latency was 6.8 0.6 s (mean 95% CI; n 41). Catching latency and breathing rate were repeatable across both trials: catching latency, R 0.31, 95% CI (0.02, 0.60), F(40, 81) 1.90, p.02; breathing rate, R 0.66, 95% CI (0.47, 0.84), F(40, 81) 4.80, p.0001. Catching latency moderately predicted breathing rate during the first trial, R 2 0.14, bootstrap 95% CI ( 0.01, 0.37), F(1, 40) 5.52, p.02, but not during the second one, R 2 0.04, bootstrap 95% CI ( 0.01, 0.19), F(1, 40) 0.01, p.99. However, overall, variation in breathing rate in relation to catching latency did not differ between the two trials: analysis of covariance, F(2, 40) 1.88, p.17. Catching latency did not differ between trials, t(1,40) 0.37, p.71, d 0.07, whereas breathing rate slightly increased from 66.2, 95% CI (64.5, 67.9) to 67.9, 95% CI (66.0, 69.7) breast movements between trials, t(40) 2.37, p.02, d 0.04. The influence of personality on breathing rate was thus analyzed after pooling data from the two trials or considering each trial separately.

HANDLING STRESS IN FEMALE ZEBRA FINCHES 3 Average breathing rate was moderately predicted by body mass, R 2 0.12, bootstrap 95% CI ( 0.01, 0.32), F(1, 40) 4.49, p.04, but not by body condition, R 2 0.04, bootstrap 95% CI ( 0.01, 0.20), F(1, 40) 0.88, p.35. Heavier individuals showed a higher breathing rate after handling. However, catching latency was not influenced by body mass, R 2 0.04, bootstrap 95% CI ( 0.01, 0.18), F(1, 40) 1.06, p.31, or body condition, R 2 0.09, bootstrap 95% CI ( 0.01, 0.26), F(1, 40) 3.40, p.07. We then computed the variables breathing rate adjusted for catching latency and breathing rate adjusted for body mass by calculating the residuals from the regressions of breathing rate on catching latency and breathing rate on body mass. None of the four personality traits was significantly related to breathing rate, breathing rate adjusted for mass or for catching latency when considering the two trials separately (see Table 1). In addition, none of the four personality traits was significantly related to breathing rate adjusted for mass averaged across the two trials: activity, r(39) 0.12, 95% CI ( 0.41, 0.19), p.45; reaction to startle, r(39) 0.06, 95% CI ( 0.25, 0.36), p.72; exploration, r(39) 0.09, 95% CI ( 0.39, 0.22), p.56; neophobia, r(39) 0.16, 95% CI ( 0.45, 0.15), p.31 (Figure 1). Discussion This study investigated whether breathing rate during handling can be a reliable predictor of personality in zebra finches. Response to handling stress in the form of breathing rate was found to be consistent within individuals. However, our results indicate that individual variation in response to handling stress is not associated to individual variation in personality in female zebra finches, as least as assessed from the methodology used in David et al. (2011), thus, casting doubts on the extent to which response to handling stress should be used as a proxy to assess personality. In particular, we found no evidence for a relationship between exploratory tendency or neophobia and breathing rate, contrary to what has been previously reported in great tits (Carere & van Oers, 2004; Fucikova et al., 2009). The same was true of two other different personality traits, activity and reaction to startle. In addition, contrary to what was observed in great tits (Carere & van Oers, 2004), we found that breathing rate was positively correlated with body mass in female zebra finches, such that heavier individuals had a higher breathing rate during handling. This might be explained by the tight relationship between metabolism and body mass (Clarke & Johnston, 1999; McNab, 2002) and by the fact that metabolic rate depends on oxygen consumption and is directly related to ventilation rate in passerines (Arens & Cooper, 2005). The absence of a relationship between personality and breathing rate is particularly interesting as some evidence suggests that basal metabolic rate is heritable in zebra finches (Rønning, Moe, & Bech, 2005). Indeed, personality traits are supposed to be independent from any motivational or life-history state (Réale et al., 2007), although few studies verified this assumption so far (Fucikova et al., 2009). Several explanations, based on methodological issues or biological differences between species, can however be advanced to explain the observed discrepancy between our results and those obtained in great tits (Carere & van Oers, 2004; Fucikova et al., 2009). From a methodological point of view, this study only used females as study subjects. The possibility then remains that breathing rate is related to male personality but not to female one, although this seems unlikely. In addition, Carere and van Oers (2004) estimated breathing rate in great tits during a 60-s period, which is substantially longer than in this study. Nonetheless, we believe that this discrepancy cannot be responsible for the results we obtained, as zebra finches get stressed from both human presence and catching. Breathing rate is, thus, likely to reflect a stable stress state in our experiment rather than the escalation of stress. Finally, Carere and van Oers (2004) assessed individual personality only from reaction of birds toward a novel object. Because Table 1 Relationships Between Each Personality Trait and Breathing Rate, Breathing Rate Adjusted for Mass, and for Catching Latency Exploration Activity Neophobia Reaction to startle r p r p r p r p Variable (95% CI) (95% CI) (95% CI) (95% CI) Breathing rate 1st trial 0.17 0.29 0.11 0.51 0.16 0.31 0.17 0.28 ( 0.45, 0.15) ( 0.40, 0.21) ( 0.45, 0.15) ( 0.14,0.46) 2nd trial 0.11 0.49 0.06 0.70 0.16 0.31 0.10 0.54 ( 0.40, 0.20) ( 0.36, 0.25) ( 0.45, 0.15) ( 0.22, 0.39) Breathing rate adjusted for mass 1st trial 0.13 0.42 0.13 0.43 0.15 0.34 0.12 0.47 ( 0.42, 0.18) ( 0.42, 0.19) ( 0.44, 0.16) ( 0.20, 0.41) 2nd trial 0.04 0.80 0.10 0.54 0.15 0.35 0.01 0.96 ( 0.34, 0.27) ( 0.39, 0.22) ( 0.44, 0.17) ( 0.31, 0.30) Breathing rate adjusted for catching latency 1st trial 0.15 0.36 0.02 0.90 0.02 0.89 0.08 0.64 ( 0.44, 0.17) ( 0.29, 0.33) ( 0.32, 0.29) ( 0.24, 0.38) 2nd trial 0.11 0.49 0.06 0.70 0.16 0.31 0.10 0.54 ( 0.40, 0.20) ( 0.36, 0.25) ( 0.45, 0.15) ( 0.22, 0.39) Note. CI confidence interval. Breathing rate adjusted for mass and adjusted for catching latency was computed as the residuals of the linear regression of breathing rate on respectively body mass and catching latency. Degrees of freedom equal 40 for each test.

4 DAVID, AUCLAIR, DECHAUME-MONCHARMONT, AND CÉZILLY Figure 1. Nonsignificant relationship between each personality traits and breathing rate (averaged across the two trials) adjusted for mass during handling. Breathing rate adjusted for mass was computed as the residuals of the linear regression of breathing rate on body mass. animal personality is most probably composed of several dimensions, it might be difficult in practice to assess it from one single trait, and inference of this sort should then be performed with extreme caution in future studies. For instance, in zebra finches, social dominance within groups is influenced by a set of personality traits such as exploration or activity (David et al., 2011). Conversely, obstinacy, which is not related to other personality traits but shows high repeatability, has no effect on social dominance in this species (David et al., 2011). Another possibility is that the relationship between personality and physiological traits may vary between species. Great tits are obligate cavity nesters, usually nesting in a hole in a tree, whereas zebra finches are not (Zann, 1996; see however Griffith, Pryke, & Mariette, 2008). Some evidence suggests that chronic exposition to CO 2 during development may affect the development of ventilatory behavior in cavity-nesting bird species (Williams & Kilgore, 1992), eventually affecting the observed relationship between personality and breathing rate in adult birds. Whatever the causes of observed discrepancies between studies, additional work is clearly needed to assess the extent to which breathing rate can be used as a reliable proxy for overall personality in great tits and other species. References Arens, J. R., & Cooper, S. J. (2005). Metabolic and ventilatory acclimatization to cold stress in house sparrows (Passer domesticus). Physiological & Biochemical Zoology, 78, 579 589. Carere, C., & van Oers, K. (2004). Shy and bold great tits (Parus major): Body temperature and breath rate in response to handling stress. Physiology & Behavior, 82, 905 912. Carere, C., Welink, D., Drent, P. J., Koolhaas, J. M., & Groothuis, T. G. G. (2001). Effect of social defeat in a territorial bird (Parus major) selected for different coping styles. Physiology & Behavior, 73, 427 433. Clarke, A., & Johnston, N. M. (1999). Scaling of metabolic rate with body mass and temperature in teleost fish. Journal of Animal Ecology, 68, 893 905. David, M., Auclair, Y., & Cézilly, F. (2011). Personality predicts social dominance in female zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata, in a feeding context. Animal Behaviour, 81, 219 224. Fucikova, E., Drent, P. J., Smits, N., & van Oers, K. (2009). Handling stress as a measurement of personality in great tit nestlings (Parus major). Ethology, 115, 366 374. Griffith, S. C., Pryke, S. R., & Mariette, M. (2008). Use of nest-boxes by the Zebra Finch (Taeniopygia guttata): Implications for reproductive success and research. Emu, 108, 311 319. Lessells, C. M., & Boag, P. T. (1987). Unrepeatable repeatabilities: A common mistake. The Auk, 104, 116 121. McNab, B. K. (2002). The physiological ecology of vertebrates: A view from energetics. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Nakagawa, S., & Cuthill, I. C. (2007). Effect size, confidence interval and statistical significance: A practical guide for biologists. Biological Reviews, 82, 591 605. Nakagawa, S., & Schielzeth, H. (2010). Repeatability for Gaussian and non-gaussian data: A practical guide for biologists. Biological Reviews, 85, 935 956. Quinn, J. L., Patrick, S. C., Bouwhuis, S., Wilkin, T. A., & Sheldon, B. C.

HANDLING STRESS IN FEMALE ZEBRA FINCHES 5 (2009). Heterogeneous selection on a heritable temperament trait in a variable environment. Journal of Animal Ecology, 78, 1203 1215. Réale, D., Reader, S. M., Sol, D., McDougall, P. T., & Dingemanse, N. J. (2007). Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biological Reviews, 82, 291 318. Rønning, B., Moe, B., & Bech, C. (2005). Long-term repeatability makes basal metabolic rate a likely heritable trait in the zebra finch Taeniopygia guttata. Journal of Experimental Biology, 208, 4663 4669. Schamber, J. L., Esler, D., & Flint, P. L. (2009). Evaluating the validity of using unverified indices of body condition. Journal of Avian Biology, 40, 49 56. Schuett, W., Tregenza, T., & Dall, S. R. X. (2010). Sexual selection and animal personality. Biological Reviews, 85, 217 246. Sih, A., Bell, A., & Johnson, J. C. (2004). Behavioral syndromes: An ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 19, 372 378. Smith, B. R., & Blumstein, D. T. (2008). Fitness consequences of personality: A meta-analysis. Behavioral Ecology, 19, 448 455. Williams, B. R., & Kilgore, D. L. (1992). Ontogenic modification of the hypercapnic ventilatory response in the zebra finch. Respiration Physiology, 90, 123 134. Wilson, A. D. M., Whatam, E. M., Bennett, R., Visanuvimol, L., Lauzon, C., & Bertram, S. M. (2010). Behavioral correlations across activity, mating, exploration, aggression, and anti-predator contexts in the European house cricket, Acheta domesticus. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 64, 703 715. Zann, R. A. (1996). The zebra finch: A synthesis of field and laboratory studies. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Received February 5, 2011 Revision received May 26, 2011 Accepted May 31, 2011