Short Communications 517

Similar documents
Status of introduced vertebrates in Galapagos Gustavo Jiménez-Uzcátegui a, Víctor Carrión b, Jabi Zabala a, Paola Buitrón a & Bryan Milstead a

HOW DO I GET TO THE GALAPAGOS ISLANDS? WHICH GALAPAGOS CRUISE ITINERARY IS RIGHT FOR ME? WHAT OTHER USEFUL GALAPAGOS INFORMATION IS AVAILABLE TO HELP

GALAPAGOS NATURE CALENDAR. Sea lions start pupping. Mums are aggresive at this time. Marine Iguanas start nesting on Santa Cruz Island

Darwin s Finches and Natural Selection

BREEDING AND FEEDING ECOLOGY OF THE GALAPAGOS DOVE

The Galapagos Islands: Crucible of Evolution.

Biology of the Galapagos

Charles Darwin. The Theory of Evolution

The Theory of Evolution

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

AP Biology. AP Biology

Darwin s. Finches. Beyond the Book. FOCUS Book

Evolution by Natural Selection

Chapter 22 Darwin and Evolution by Natural Selection

Evolution by Natural Selection

Evolution by Natural Selection

Biology of the Galapagos

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns

144 Short Communications [Auk, Vol. 106

Evolution and Natural Selection. Peekskill High School Biology by: First-name Last-name

Life s Natural History = a record of Successions & Extinctions. Anaerobic Bacteria. Photosynthetic Bacteria. Green Algae. Multicellular Animals

DO BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS?

Red-Tailed Hawk Buteo jamaicensis

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

Cooperative Breeding by the Galfipagos Mockingbird, Nesomimus parvulus

Darwin s Finches: A Thirty Year Study.

Great Horned Owl (Bubo virginianus) Productivity and Home Range Characteristics in a Shortgrass Prairie. Rosemary A. Frank and R.

Andros Iguana Education Kit Checklist

COULD YOU HAVE RIDDEN A HORSE MILLIONS OF YEARS AGO? Horse evolution goes back more than 55 million years

Demography and breeding success of Falklands skua at Sea Lion Island, Falkland Islands

PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE

FOOD HABITS OF NESTING COOPER S HAWKS AND GOSHAWKS IN NEW YORK AND PENNSYLVANIA

2 How Does Evolution Happen?

EIDER JOURNEY It s Summer Time for Eiders On the Breeding Ground

The Divergence of the Marine Iguana: Amblyrhyncus cristatus. from its earlier land ancestor (what is now the Land Iguana). While both the land and

Lonesome George: RIP. Galápagos tortoises

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

King penguin brooding and defending a sub-antarctic skua chick

Chapter 4 Nesting Chronology Of The Marbled Murrelet

Disappearing Marine Iguanas: A Case of Population Collapse

Influence of kinship on helping behavior in Galfipagos mockingbirds

We are adult American. Field Marks. We are the smallest falcons in North America. Like other falcons, we have long, pointed wings,

08 alberts part2 7/23/03 9:10 AM Page 95 PART TWO. Behavior and Ecology

SEASONAL PATTERNS OF NESTING IN THE RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD MORTALITY

Welcome to Darwin Day!

Wilson Bull., 94(2), 1982, pp

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

Disappearing Marine Iguanas: A Case of Population Collapse

CHICK GROWTH, SIBLING RIVALRY, AND CHICK

Atlantic Puffins By Guy Belleranti

From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research. Animal behavior then & now

Evolution of Birds. Summary:

Evolution. Evolution is change in organisms over time. Evolution does not have a goal; it is often shaped by natural selection (see below).

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Laying May May 2 to 26. Incubation Early May to mid June Early May to mid June 30 to 34

Notes on Varanus salvator marmoratus on Polillo Island, Philippines. Daniel Bennett.

Removal of Alaskan Bald Eagles for Translocation to Other States Michael J. Jacobson U.S Fish and Wildlife Service, Juneau, AK

BREEDING ECOLOGY OF THE LITTLE TERN, STERNA ALBIFRONS PALLAS, 1764 IN SINGAPORE

Unit 7: Adaptation STUDY GUIDE Name: SCORE:

Florida Field Naturalist

AGENDA 1. Natural Selection Activity HOMEWORK 1. Notebook!

Iguana Technical Assistance Workshop. Presented by: Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

OBSERVATIONS OF HAWAIIAN

FEATURED PHOTO NOTES ON PLUMAGE MATURATION IN THE RED-TAILED TROPICBIRD

AGE AT FIRST BREEDING AND CHANGE IN PLUMAGE OF KELP GULLS LARUS DOMINICANUS IN SOUTH AFRICA. R. J. M. CRAWFORD*, B. M. DYER* and L.

Systematics, Taxonomy and Conservation. Part I: Build a phylogenetic tree Part II: Apply a phylogenetic tree to a conservation problem

Macroevolution Part II: Allopatric Speciation

PROJECT REPORT GALAPAGOS ISLANDS VOLUNTEER TRIP NOVEMBER 12-22, 2017 Executive Summary

Ardea herodias (Great Blue Heron)

DISPERSAL IN THE COMMUNALLY BREEDING GROOVE-BILLED AN1 (CROTOPHAGA SULCIROSTRIS)

Scholarship 2016 Biology

Everyday Mysteries: Why most male birds are more colorful than females

Reduced availability of refuse and breeding output in a herring gull (Larus argentatus) colony

Conservation issues for Darwin s finches in. the Galápagos Islands: invasive species and. loss of genetic diversity

EXERCISE 14 Marine Birds at Sea World Name

By Hans Frey ¹ ² & Alex Llopis ²

Hawks Order Falconiformes

Coccyzus minor (Mangrove Cuckoo)

Student Exploration: Rainfall and Bird Beaks

Plestiodon (=Eumeces) fasciatus Family Scincidae

Sparrowhawks & Goshawks and the Gymnogene

Larkey Genetics Code

Rock Wren Nesting in an Artificial Rock Wall in Folsom, Sacramento County, California

Reading Science! Name: Date: Darwin s Fancy with Finches Lexile 1190L

Island Fox Update 2011

A.13 BLAINVILLE S HORNED LIZARD (PHRYNOSOMA BLAINVILLII)

The tailed frog has been found from sea level to near timberline ( m; Province of BC 1999).

COOPERATIVE BREEDING IN THE TROPICAL MOCKINGBIRD (MIMUS GILVUS) IN THE PANAMA CANAL ZONE

A.13 BLAINVILLE S HORNED LIZARD (PHRYNOSOMA BLAINVILLII)

Darwin s Theory of Evolution Chapter 16

Coyote (Canis latrans)

Who Am I? Animal Book. 3rd Grade Research Class

ANALYSIS OF GROWTH OF THE RED-TAILED HAWK 1

Breeding White Storks( Ciconia ciconia at Chessington World of Adventures Paul Wexler

! Three things needed to survive on land were: ! 1. Have lungs and breathe air. ! 2. Have a body resistant to drying out.

18th century - first to propose that could change over time and that changes could - proposed that relatively had formed many through.

State birds. A comparison of the Northern Mockingbird and the Western Meadowlark. By Shaden Jensen

ISSN Department of Conservation. Reference to material in this report should be cited thus:

Growth and Development. Embryonic development 2/22/2018. Timing of hatching. Hatching. Young birds and their parents

Transcription:

July 1987] Short Communications 517 Interisland Variation in Blood Drinking by Galhpagos Mockingbirds ROBERT L. CURRY AND DAVID J. ANDERSON Department of Biology, The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109-1048 USA Few species of birds are known to feed on the blood of living animals, and the occurrence of this behavior in populations of the Sharp-beaked Ground-Finch (Geospiza difficilis) on Wolf and Darwin islands, Gal tpagos, has become notorious (Bowman and Billeb 1965, KiSster and KiSster 1983, Schluter and Grant 1984). It is less widely recognized, however, that Galhpagos mockingbirds (Nesomimuspp.) on some islands also drink blood from living hosts. Occasional blood drinking by mockingbirds on two islands was mentioned previously, but without details on the frequency or distribution of the trait (Bowman and Carter 1971, Christian 1980). We observed blood drinking during studies of mockingbird cooperative breeding (Curry) and booby breeding biology (Anderson) between 1981 and 1985. Four endemic, allopatric mockingbird species inhabit the archipelago (Swarth 1931). We studied N. macdonaldi on Espafiola; N. trifasciatus, the Floreana (Charles Island) Mockingbird, on Champion and Gardner-by-Floreana; N. melanotis, the San Crist6bal (Chatham Island) Mockingbird, on San Crist bal; and N. parvulus, the Gal pagos Mockingbird, on Darwin, Fernandina, Genovesa, Isabela, Santa Cruz, Santa Fe, Santiago, and Wolf. We studied color-banded birds on Genovesa (1981-1985), Champion (1981-1985), Espafiola (1984-1985), and San Crist bal (1984). On the other islands we observed unmarked birds during brief visits. Mockingbirds (N. macdonaldi) on Espafiola foraged in all habitats, eating fruit and both terrestrial and marine arthropods; the diet of mockingbirds on other islands is similar (Grant and Grant 1979, Curry unpubl. data). We observed Espafiola mockingbirds drinking blood from living sea lions (Zalophus californianus), marine iguanas (Amblyrhynchusubcristatus), and nestling Masked Boobies (Sula dactylatra) in two locations near Punta Cevallos. Mockingbirds also twice attempted to drink blood from superficial wounds on the legs of field investigators. On 27 December 1983 a wounded bull sea lion pulled out on an Espafiola beach after receiving severe lacerations in its genital area during a fight with another male. Between 1400 and dusk at 1800 six banded mockingbirds drank blood directly from the sea lion's wounds while it lay on the beach. The bull left during the night, and the birds pecked at blood on the sand the next day. Mockingbirds also frequently obtained blood from sea lion placentas in one coastal area where Present address: Department of Biology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104 USA. many sea lions gave birth. Up to four birds at a time fed from each placenta, eating tissues and drinking from pools of blood on the rocks. We saw Espafiola mockingbirds drink blood from living marine iguanas on four different occasions, all in 1984, in sections of rocky coast where iguanas were abundant. In each case the bird fed from a small hole in the iguana's tail until the victim moved into a protected position. Nesomimus macdonaldz may create these wounds incidentally while tearing off and eating ticks or skin from marine iguanas, as sometimes occurs when N. parvulus pecks ticks from land iguanas on Santa Fe (Christian 1980). On Espafiola 25% of 100 iguanas examined had wounds like those from which we saw mockingbirds drink blood. In contrast, we found no such wounds on 100 iguanas at Isla Lobos and Cerro Brujo, San Crist6bal, where we did not see mockingbirds glean ticks or drink blood. We also saw several N. macdonaldi drink blood from the ground Fig. 1. Mockingbird (Nesomimus macdonaldi) drinking blood from a conspicuous wound in the neck of a living Masked Booby (Sula dactylatra) nestling on Isla Espafiola, Gal tpagos, while standing on the victim's head.

518 Short Communications [Auk, Vol. 104 T^I LE 1. Blood drinking and associated behavior among Gal pagos mockingbird (Nesomimus spp.) populations. Blood drinking includes only blood taken from living hosts; iguana pecking includes mockingbirds gleaning ticks or skin; carrion feeding includes only vertebrate sources. Host organisms are abbreviated as follows: marine iguanas (MI), land iguanas (LI), lava lizards or geckos (LG), seabirds (SB), sea lions (SL), tortoises (T), and goats (G). Behavior not observed on well-studied islands is indicated by a dash. Mockingbird behavior remains poorly known on islands indicated by a question mark. Sources of observations are denoted by footnotes. Iguana Species Island Blood drinking pecking Carrion feeding N. macdonaldi N. melanotis N. trifasciatus N. parvulus This study. Bowman and Carter (1971). Amadon (1966). Carpenter (1966). ' Gifford (1919). Venables (1940). Bowman (pers. comm.). Harris (1968). Beebe (1924); population now extinct. Christian (1980). Schluter (pers. comm.). A. Laurie (pers. comm.). Darwin (1841). Espafiola,b'c'a' SL, SB, MI MI, LG LG, MI, SB, G San Crist6bab f, -- MI -- Champion a -- -- LG, SL, SB Gardner-by-Floreana,h?? SB Baltra i?? LI Darwin a _?? Fernandina 'b, -- MI, LI LI Genovesa a -- -- SB Isabela a'?? LG Marchena a,k -- MI LG Seymour??? Pinta k --? LG R bida??? Santa Cruz a,b -- -- LG Santa Fe a,b,i,l MI, LI MI, LI MI, G, SL Santiago a'm?? T Wolf a --?? after a Gal pagos Hawk (Buteo galapagoensis) killed with their bills. Parental boobies also defended their and removed an iguana (see Bowman and Carter 1971). chicks and occasionally may have killed blood drink- Espafiola mockingbirds obtained blood more fre- ers: we found the bodies of four mockingbirds, inquently from seabirds than from sea lions or iguanas. cluding a banded individual that previously drank We observed mockingbirds drinking blood from 9 blood from a booby nestling, next to nests containing different Masked Booby nestlings (25-50 days old) in booby chicks. During long intervals when booby par- 1984 and from another 22 in 1985, in an area at Punta ents left the nest to forage, blood drinking by mock- Cevallos of roughly 300 booby nests. Espafiola mock- ingbirds was interrupted only when nonparental ingbirds also attack Blue-footed Boobies (S. nebouxii; boobies resumed their assaults on the chicks. Attack- Nelson 1968) and Waved Albatross (Diomedea irrorata) ing mockingbirds gathered around each victim and chicks (S. Harcourt pers. comm.), but we saw no mock- attempted to perch on its back or head to eat tissue ingbirds drink blood from these species. Most, if not and drink blood (Fig. 1). In a few cases we saw only all, of the 31 victims had been assaulted by nonpar- one mockingbird drink blood from a chick, but usuental adult Masked Boobies, resulting in abrasions ally several did so (mean minimum number of and lacerations on the chicks' necks, heads, rumps, feeders = 4.3 + 5.1 SD, n = 28). As each victim's and wings. We do not know why these assaults (Nel- condition deteriorated and it became increasingly unson 1978: 385) occurred, but the wounds clearly at- responsive, more blood drinkers joined in. In one case tracted mockingbirds. [Finches(G. difficilis) on Wolf at least 35 mockingbirds fed from a single booby nestfeed from booby nestlings wounded in the same way ling just before it died. Most victims died within 24 (K/Sster and K/Sster 1983).] When first attacked, booby h of the onset of blood drinking, usually after their chicks deterred mockingbirds temporarily by jabbing vertebral columns and spinal cords were exposed.

July 1987] Short Communications 519 At least 38% of the 55 mockingbirds resident at Punta Cevallos drank booby blood one or more times, tions about the origin of these unusual traits. It is not surprising that some Gal pagos mockingbirds should and some individuals alternated between two victims drink blood because all forage opportunistically (Beebe in a single day. Birds from other parts of the island 1924, Amadon 1966, Bowman and Carter 1971, Grant where seabirds did not nest also drank blood from and Grant 1979, Curry 1986), in common with the few boobies at Punta Cevallos; three of the banded N. macdonaldi we saw drinking blood held territories 2 km away. Our data are insufficient to determine what proportion of the diet of mockingbirds on Espaf ola consists of blood. The fact that mockingbirds avidly exploit all available sources of blood suggests that blood is a preferred food. Blood drinking may occur predominantly during dry seasons when other foods are scarce. The blood drinking we observed took place during dry periods in both 1984 and 1985. Blood drinking was less frequent under wetter conditions in 1986 (Anderson pers. obs.). other species of blood-drinking birds (Bowman and Billeb 1965). This factor does not explain why blood drinking is restricted to just a few populations. Unlike egg eating by Gal pagos mockingbirds (Bowman and Carter 1971), there is no simple correspondence between blood drinking and ecological setting, mockingbird morphology, or the distribution of hosts among the islands. Espafiola, Santa Fe, and Genovesa are small, low, and arid islands where mockingbirds are among the longest-billed forms (Swarth 1931). Blood drinking occurs on Espafiola and Santa Fe but not on Genovesa. Potential sources, including sea lions (Eibl-Eibesfeldt 1984a), iguanas (Eibl- Information available about other islands indicates Eibesfeldt 1984b), and nesting seabirds (Harris 1973), that there is considerable interisland variability in blood drinking and related behavior among Gal - pagos mockingbird populations (Table 1). In Table 1 we have listed all known records of blood drinking from living hosts; we also include observations of mockingbirds removing ticks or skin from iguanas ("iguana pecking") or feeding on carrion, because most of the few other known blood-eating bird species feed on ectoparasites or carrion, or both (Bowman and Billeb 1965). After Espafiola, blood drinking occurs most frequently on Santa Fe, where mockingbirds (N. parvulus) occasionally drink blood from land iguanas occur virtually throughout the archipelago. Additional factors must be invoked to explain why blood drinking is not common to all arid-island populations. Variation in social behavior among Gal pagos mockingbird populations may account partially for the distribution of blood drinking. If specialized feeding techniques, including blood drinking, are learned (Bowman and Billeb 1965, Christian 1980), such traits may become established in socially complex populations in which opportunities for learning occur frequently, as suggested for tick pecking in Aphelocoma (Conolophus pallidus; Christian 1980) and from marine coerulescens (Baber and Morris 1980, Isenhart and iguanas wounded by hawks or sharks (A. Laurie pers. comm.). Blood drinking is not known to occur in any DeSante 1985). Gal pagos mockingbirds have complex social organizations that include group territoother Gal pagos mockingbird population. Nesomimus riality and cooperative breeding (Hatch 1966, Grant parvulus on Genovesa has not been seen drinking blood and Grant 1979, Kinnaird and Grant 1982, Curry 1987). from any living hosts, though mockingbirds have been With some variation these factors correlate with blood studied intensively there since 1978 using field methods identical to those we used on Espafiola (Grant and Grant 1979, Kinnaird and Grant 1982, Curry 1985, pers. obs.). Anderson saw finches (G. difficilis), but no mockingbirds, drink blood from seabirds on Wolf and drinking. Mockingbirds in coastal areas of Espafiola, where blood drinking is most frequent, live in large groups (up to 17 birds) at high density (9-16 birds/ ha); on Santa Crist6bal, where blood drinking is not known to occur, mockingbirds live in much smaller Darwin in 1981. We found no evidence of blood groups (2-3 birds) at lower density (less than 1 bird/ drinking in N. parvulus on Santiago, Isabela, Fernandina, or Santa Cruz. Mockingbirds (N. trifasciatus) on Champion feed on carrion, and on Gardner-by-Floreana they occasionally kill seabird chicks (Harris 1968), but we did not observe iguana pecking or blood drinking on either islet. No blood drinking or carrion feeding has been reported in N. melanotis. The birds we studied at three locations on San Crist6bal (Playa Baquerizo, Cerro Brujo, E1 Junco) foraged arboreally and did not attempt to feed from iguanas or seabirds, but Bowman (pers. comm.) saw N. melanotis pecking ticks from marine iguanas at another location (Punta ha) (Curry 1987). However, mockingbirds live in moderately large groups (averaging 3-4 birds) at high density (4 birds/ha) on Champion and Genovesa (Curry 1987) but do not drink blood, so social complexity alone does not fully account for the distribution of blood drinking. We suggest that interisland variation in blood drinking may result from differences in the frequency of opportunities to sample blood, which could arise in at least two ways. First, blood should be more often available to mockingbirds on islands where Gal pagos Hawks kill or wound large prey such as iguanas Pitt). or seabirds than on islands without hawks. A role for The occurrence of blood drinking and associated behavior by mockingbirds on some islands, combined with their apparent absence on others, raises queshawks in mockingbird blood drinking is supported by the observation that hawks are resident on Espafiola and Santa Fe, where mockingbirds drink blood,

520 Short Communications [Auk, Vol. 104 but hawks are absent from Genovesa, Champion, Darwin, R bida, San Crist( bal, Santa Cruz, Seymour, and Wolf (Harris 1973), islands where mockingbirds are not known to drink blood. Blood drinking by mockingbirds could be expected on other islands with resident Gal pagos Hawks (Isabela, Fernandina, Marchena, Pinta, and Santiago), but foraging patterns in these mockingbird populations are not sufficiently well known to test this prediction. Mockingbirds also might be exposed to blood from smaller animals killed by Short-eared Owls (Asio fiammeus) or Common Barn- Owls (Tyto alba). The distribution of neither of these predators, however, coincides with the pattern of blood drinking among mockingbird populations. Asio inhabits all principal islands, including those where mockingbirds do and do not drink blood, and Tyto is absent from Espafiola, where blood drinking by mockingbirds is most conspicuous. Mockingbirds could be differentially exposed to blood through iguana pecking. Mockingbirds that glean ectoparasites from iguanas would have frequent opportunities to sample blood. Generalized blood drinking may have derived from a protocooperative association between mockingbirds and iguanas (Christian 1980, Isenhart and DeSante 1985; see also Bowman and Billeb 1965). Consistent with this hypothesis, mockingbirds most frequently drink blood on islands where they also peck at iguanas (Table 1), although mockingbirds have been seen pecking at iguanas on two other islands (Fernandina and San Crist6bal) where no blood feeding has been reported. Furthermore, the hypothesis fails to explain why the tendency to peck at iguanas varies among mockingbird populations. Unless additional factors can account for variation in iguana pecking by mockingbirds, the hypothesis that hawks indirectly influence mockingbird blood drinking is better supported by the information available. It remains possible, however, that blood drinking arose simply by chance in just two populations. Regardless of which explanation for the distribution of blood drinking is correct, N. macdonaldi on Espafiola has evolved on exceptional tendency to exploit blood from any available source. This research was carried out in conjunction with P. R. Grant's studies of Gal pagos land birds; we thank him for help throughout the study and for comments on an earlier draft. We thank R. I. Bowman for con- structive criticism and his unpublished observation of iguana pecking on San Crist bal. R. Brubaker, B. Coffman, S. Curry, S. Forther, L. Hamilton, M. Iturralde, S. Stoleson, and S. Webb provided field assistance. The Charles Darwin Research Station helped with logistics, and World Wildlife Fund-US, the Frank M. Chapman Memorial Fund, the George D. Harris Foundation, Sigma Xi, and the University of Michi- gan contributed financial support. Permission for this study was granted by the Direcci6n General de Desarollo Forestal, Ecuador, and the Gal pagos National Park Service. We thank Superintendent Miguel Cifuentes for his support. LITERATURE CITED AMADON, D. 1966. Insular adaptive radiation among birds. Pp. 18-30 in The Gal pagos (R. I. Bowman, Ed.). Berkeley, Univ. California Press. BABER, D. W., & J. G. MORRIS. 1980. Florida Scrub Jays foraging from feral hogs. Auk 97: 202. BEEBE, W. 1924. Gal pagos: world's end. New York, Putnam's Sons. BOWMAN, R. I., & S. L. BILLEB. 1965. Blood-eating in a Gal pagos finch. Living Bird 4: 29-44., & A. CARTER. 1971. Egg-pecking behavior in Galfipagos mockingbirds. Living Bird 10: 243-270. CARPENTER, C. C. 1966. The marine iguana of the Gal pagos Islands: its behavior and ecology. Proc. California Acad. Sci., Ser. 4, 34: 329-375. CHRISTIAN, K.A. 1980. Cleaning/feeding symbiosis between birds and reptiles of the Gal pagos Islands: new observations of inter-island variabil- ity. Auk 97: 887-889. CURRY, R. L. 1985. Breeding and survival of Gal - pagos mockingbirds during E1 Nifio. Pp. 449-471 in E1 Nifio en las Islas Gal pagos: el evento de 1982-1983 (G. Robinson and E. del Pino, Eds.). Quito, Ecuador, Fundaci6n Charles Darwin para las Islas Gal pagos. 1986. Gal pagos mockingbird kleptoparasitizes centipede. Condor 88: 119-120. 1987. Evolution and ecology of cooperative breeding in Gal pagos mockingbirds (Nesomimus spp.). Ph.D. dissertation, Ann Arbor, Univ. Michigan. DARWIN, C. R. (Ed.). 1841. Zoology of the voyage of H. M. S. Beagle, under the command of Captain FitzRoy, R. N., during the years 1832-1836. Part III: Birds. London, Smith Elder & Co. EIBL-EIBESFELDT, I. 1984a. The Gal pagos seals. Part 1, Natural history of the Gal pagos sea lion (Zalophus californianus wollebaecki, Sivertsen). Pp. 207-214 in Key environments: Gal pagos (R. Perry, Ed.). New York, Pergamon. 1984b. The large iguanas of the Gal pagos Islands. Pp. 157-173 in Key environments: Galfipagos (R. Perry, Ed.). New York, Pergamon. GIFFORD, E. W. 1919. Field notes on the land birds of the Gal pagos Islands and of Cocos Island, Costa Rica. Proc. California Acad. Sci. 2, Ser. 4: 189-258. GRANT, P. R., & N. GRANT. 1979. Breeding and feeding of Gal pagos Mockingbirds, Nesomimus parvulus. Auk 96: 723-736. HARRIS, M.P. 1968. Egg-eating by Galfipagos mockingbirds. Condor 70: 269-270. 1973. The Gal pagos avifauna. Condor 75: 265-278. HATCH, J.H. 1966. Collective territories in Gal pa-

July 1987] Short Communications 521 gos mockingbirds, with notes on other behavior. Wilson Bull. 78: 198-206. ISENHART, F. R., 8t D. F. DESANTE. 1985. Observations of Scrub Jays cleaning ectoparasites from blacktailed deer. Condor 87: 145-147. KINNAIRD, M. F., & P. R. GRANT. 1982ß Cooperative breeding in the Galfipagos Mockingbird, Nesomimus parvulus. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 10: 65-73. K SSTER, F., & H. K SSTER. 1983. Twelve days among the "vampire finches" of Wolf Island. Not. Galfipagos 38: 4-10. NELSON, B. 1968. Gal pagos, islands of birds. New York, William Morrow and Co. ß 1978. The Sulidae. Oxford, Aberdeen Univ. Press. SCHLUTER, D., 8t P. R. GRANTß 1984. Ecological correlates of morphological evolution in a Darwin's finch, Geospiza difficilis. Evolution 38: 856-869. SWARTH, H.S. 1931. The avifauna of the Gal pagos Islands. Occ. Pap. California Acad. Sci. 18: 1-299. VENABLES, L. S. V. 1940. Nesting behaviour of the Gal pagos Mockingbird. Ibis 82: 629-639. Received 8 August 1986, accepted 16 January 1987. Further Evidence of Long-term Pair Bonds in Ducks of the Genus Bucephala GILLES GAUTHIER 1 Department of Zoology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1W5, Canada In holarctic ducks, pair formation occurs during the winter or the spring migration, or both, and pair bonds are seasonal, usually lasting until the onset of incubation by the female (McKinney 1986). Because males desert their mates during incubation and leave the breeding grounds for molting, it has been thought that new pair bonds are formed every year and that repairing with the same mate does not occur (Rowley 1983, McKinney 1986). In dabbling ducks, marking males on the breeding grounds has confirmed that long-term pair bonds (>1 yr) do not occur (Poston 1974, Blohm 1978), although exceptions exist (e.g. Dwyer et al. 1973). In seaducks, however, there is evidence that pairing with the same individual for more than one breeding season may be a common occurrence. Savard (1985a) recently reported that in Barrow's Goldeneye (Bucephala islandica) the return rates of adult males and females to the breeding grounds were similar (66% vs. 76%), and that several pairs reunited in subsequent years. I now present evidence that male Buffleheads (B. albeola) are also philopatric to their breeding area and that pair bonds can reform in subsequent years. The study was conducted in the Cariboo Parkland of British Columbia. In 1983 I captured 3 male Buffieheads using a mirror trap (Savard 1985b), whereas 66 females were trapped on the nest between 1982 and 1985. All birds were marked individually with color-coded nasal saddles and a set of color bands. In Present address: D partement de Biologie, Universit Laval, Ste. Foy, Quebec G1K 7P4, Canada. all cases the mates of marked males were marked. Retention of saddles was poor after 2 yr, but the leg bands enabled me to identify individuals up to at least 4 yr. Pair 1 consisted of the same individuals in 1983 and 1984. This pair was not seen on the study area in 1985, but female 1 returned in 1986 with a new mate. From 1982 to 1984, and again in 1986, female 1 nested successfully in the same tree cavity, and her mate always defended the same territory, located on the pond closest to her nest. The reasons for her absence in 1.985 are unknown as both the cavity and territory were unoccupied. Pair 2 involved the same male and female for at least three consecutive seasons. Female 2 failed to hatch a clutch in all years and changed nest sites every year. The pair changed ponds from 1983 to 1984 but not in 1985. In 1986 male 2 returned unpaired for a fourth year to the same pond. His former mate did not return to the study area. For much of the nesting season he remained on the same pond, where he joined transient groups of nonbreeders and performed courtship displays to unpaired females. Pair 3 nested successfully in 1983 but was not seen subsequently on the study area. Male 3, however, was known to be alive in the following year because he was resighted on the wintering area near San Diego, California, in December 1984 (T. Meyer pers. comm.). Two of the three marked pairs, therefore, remained intact for 2 yr and one of them for a third year. Buffieheads are highly territorial during the breeding season (Gauthier 1987), and philopatry of females to the territory is high (Erskine 1961, Gauthier in prep.). My results show that philopatry may also be strong