Does dewlap size predict male bite performance in. Jamaican Anolis lizards? B. VANHOOYDONCK,* A. Y. HERREL,* R. VAN DAMME and D. J.

Similar documents
ARTICLE IN PRESS. Zoology 110 (2007) 2 8

Locomotor performance and social dominance in male Anolis cristatellus

Are morphologyperformance relationships invariant across different seasons? A test with the green anole lizard (Anolis carolinensis)

THE EFFECTS OF MORPHOLOGY AND PERCH DIAMETER ON SPRINT PERFORMANCE OF ANOLIS LIZARDS

LIZARD EVOLUTION VIRTUAL LAB

No Evidence for Female Association with High-Performance males in the Green Anole Lizard, Anolis carolinensis

What determines dewlap diversity in Anolis lizards? An among-island comparison

The Origin of Species: Lizards in an Evolutionary Tree

Chapter 16: Evolution Lizard Evolution Virtual Lab Honors Biology. Name: Block: Introduction

The Making of the Fittest: LESSON STUDENT MATERIALS USING DNA TO EXPLORE LIZARD PHYLOGENY

Signalling displays during predator prey interactions in a Puerto Rican anole, Anolis cristatellus

species: sexual dimorphisms in structure and function

An experimental test of the effect of signal size and performance capacity on dominance in the green anole lizard

Faculty Mentor, Department of Integrative Biology, Oklahoma State University

Effects of Sarcophagid Fly Infestations on Green Anole Lizards (Anolis carolinensis): An Analysis across Seasons and Age/Sex Classes

Adaptive radiation versus intraspeci c differentiation: morphological variation in Caribbean Anolis lizards

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Is it better to be bigger? Featured scientists: Aaron Reedy and Robert Cox from the University of Virginia Co-written by Matt Kustra

SEXUAL DIMORPHISM IN BODY SHAPE WITHOUT SEXUAL DIMORPHISM IN BODY SIZE IN WATER SKINKS (EULAMPRUS QUOYII)

Fight versus flight: physiological basis for temperature-dependent behavioral shifts in lizards

Effects of Hind-Limb Length and Perch Diameter on Clinging Performance in Anolis Lizards from the British Virgin Islands

Supporting Online Material for

Sex-specific evolution of bite performance in Liolaemus lizards (Iguania: Liolaemidae): the battle of the sexes

Dipsas trinitatis (Trinidad Snail-eating Snake)

Relationships between head size, bite force, prey handling. efficiency and diet in two sympatric lacertid lizards

A COMPARATIVE TEST OF ADAPTIVE HYPOTHESES FOR SEXUAL SIZE DIMORPHISM IN LIZARDS

Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles

SEVERAL fundamental studies in community ecology

Plestiodon (=Eumeces) fasciatus Family Scincidae

Sex-specific differences in ecomorphological relationships in lizards of the genus Gallotia

Appendix from T. J. Ord and J. A. Stamps, Species Identity Cues in Animal Communication

PUBLICATIONS (PEER REVIEWED)

16.3 Adaptation and Speciation in Greater Antillean Anoles

16.4 Concluding Comments

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

NATURAL AND SEXUAL VARIATION

USING DNA TO EXPLORE LIZARD PHYLOGENY

Sexual dimorphism in head shape and diet in the cottonmouth snake (Agkistrodon piscivorus)

Ethology of Crayfish. Name. Lab partners: Page 1

Differences in Visual Signal Design and Detectability between Allopatric Populations of Anolis Lizards

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

BREEDING ROBINS AND NEST PREDATORS: EFFECT OF PREDATOR TYPE AND DEFENSE STRATEGY ON INITIAL VOCALIZATION PATTERNS

Do the traits of organisms provide evidence for evolution?

Foils of flexion: the effects of perch compliance on lizard locomotion and perch choice in the wild

Everyday Mysteries: Why most male birds are more colorful than females

CAROTENOID-BASED DEWLAP COLOR AS A VISUAL SIGNAL IN SOCIAL. COMMUNICATION OF BROWN ANOLES (Norops sagrei) John Edward Steffen

CAMBRIDGE, MASS. 26 MARCH 2010 NUMBER 519 CRUISE FORAGING OF INVASIVE CHAMELEON (CHAMAELEO JACKSONII XANTHOLOPHUS) IN HAWAI I

Morphological Variation in Anolis oculatus Between Dominican. Habitats

The Incredible Shrinking Dewlap: Signal Size, Skin Elasticity, and Mechanical Design in the Green Anole Lizard (Anolis Carolinensis)

Assessing display variability in wild brown anoles Anolis sagrei using a mechanical lizard model

LIZARD ECOLOGY. D ONALD B. MILES is Professor in the Department of Biological Sciences at Ohio University.

MA41 Colour variability and the ecological use of colour in the chameleons and geckos of Mahamavo

REPTILES OF JAMAICA. Peter Vogel Department of Life Sciences Mona Campus University of the West Indies

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

CAMBRIDGE, MASS. 10 JULY 2013 NUMBER 535

J.K. McCoy CURRICULUM VITAE. J. Kelly McCoy. Department of Biology Angelo State University San Angelo, TX

SEXUAL DIMORPHISM IN HEAD SIZE IN THE LITTLE BROWN SKINK (SCINCELLA LATERALIS)

Lizard malaria: cost to vertebrate host's reproductive success

The Origin of Species: Lizards in an Evolutionary Tree

Fact Sheet: Oustalet s Chameleon Furcifer oustaleti

Ontogenetic Scaling of Bite Force in Lizards and Turtles*

Anhinga anhinga (Anhinga or Snake-bird)

08 alberts part2 7/23/03 9:10 AM Page 95 PART TWO. Behavior and Ecology

Iguana aggression. A relaxed green iguana. Defensive aggression

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc

Push and bite: trade-offs between burrowing and biting in a burrowing skink (Acontias percivali)

Push and bite: trade-offs between burrowing and biting in a burrowing skink (Acontias percivali)

LAB. NATURAL SELECTION

Crotophaga major (Greater Ani)

LABORATORY EXERCISE: CLADISTICS III. In fact, cladistics is becoming increasingly applied in a wide range of fields. Here s a sampling:

Trait compensation between boldness and the propensity for tail autotomy under different food availabilities in similarly aged brown anole lizards

Laboratory Protocols for Husbandry and Embryo Collection of Anolis Lizards

Tracing the origins of signal diversity in anole lizards: phylogenetic approaches to inferring the evolution of complex behaviour

Animal Behavior OBJECTIVES PREPARATION SCHEDULE VOCABULARY BACKGROUND INFORMATION MATERIALS. For the class. The students.

Factors influencing parasite load in male ornate tree lizards (Urosaurus ornatus): throat colour, population density, and habitat type

Name Class Date. How does a founding population adapt to new environmental conditions?

NATURAL SELECTION SIMULATION

STATUS SIGNALING IN DARK-EYED JUNCOS

8/19/2013. What is convergence? Topic 11: Convergence. What is convergence? What is convergence? What is convergence? What is convergence?

Evolution of Birds. Summary:

Dacnis cayana (Blue Dacnis or Turquoise Honeycreeper)

The purpose of this lab was to examine inheritance patters in cats through a

Introduction. Lizards: very diverse colour patterns intra- and interspecific differences in colour

A comparison of evolutionary radiations in Mainland and West Indian Anolis lizards. Ecology

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Territorial signals. Design rules for territorial signals. Why defend a territory? Bird song and territory defense

1 Describe the anatomy and function of the turtle shell. 2 Describe respiration in turtles. How does the shell affect respiration?

Sexual dimorphism in conspicuousness and ornamentation in the enigmatic leaf-nosed lizard Ceratophora tennentii from Sri Lanka

RESEARCH. Elizabeth C. Miller

Correlations between habitat use and body shape in a phrynosomatid lizard (Urosaurus ornatus): a population-level analysis

The Origin of Species: Lizards in an Evolutionary Tree

Maturity and Other Reproductive Traits of the Kanahebi Lizard Takydromus tachydromoides (Sauria, Lacertidae) in Mito

SOAR Research Proposal Summer How do sand boas capture prey they can t see?

DOES EXPERIMENTALLY INDUCED CONSPICUOUS COLORATION INCREASE RISK OF PREDATION AND CONSPECIFIC AGGRESSION IN FIRST-YEAR COLLARED LIZARD MALES?

IN species where tail autotomy can occur during

4B: The Pheasant Case: Handout. Case Three Ring-Necked Pheasants. Case materials: Case assignment

STUDIES ON THE FAUNA OF CURAÇAO AND OTHER

Obituary A Monument to Natural History Henry S. Fitch ( )

FEMALE RESPONSE TO FILMED DISPLAYS OF ANOLIS NEBULOSUS (SAURIA, IGUANIDAE)

[Source: D W Sims and V A Quayla (1998) Nature 393, pages ] (2)

T he genus Anolis (family Iguanidae or

Transcription:

Functional Ecology 2005 Does dewlap size predict male bite performance in Blackwell Publishing, Ltd. Jamaican Anolis lizards? B. VANHOOYDONCK,* A. Y. HERREL,* R. VAN DAMME and D. J. IRSCHICK Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, 310 Dinwiddie Hall, Tulane University, New Orleans LA70118, USA, and Department of Biology, University of Antwerp, Universiteitsplein 1, B-2610 Wilrijk, Belgium Summary 1. The theory of the evolution of secondary sexual traits through male male competition is based on the assumption that these traits are reliable cues for an animal s fighting capacity. In this paper, we test whether a secondary sexual trait, dewlap size, is an honest predictor of bite strength in Anolis lizards. Since male anoles will bite one another during territorial fights, bite performance may play an important role in determining the outcome of male male fights. 2. We measured dewlap size and bite force in a size series of adult males of three Jamaican anoles, i.e. Anolis grahami Gray 1845, A. lineatopus Gray 1840 and A. valencienni Duméril & Bibron 1837. 3. In both A. grahami and A. lineatopus, males with relatively large dewlaps tend to bite relatively harder, while the relationship between relative dewlap size and bite force was clearly non-significant within the twig anole A. valencienni. 4. Our results thus suggest that dewlap size is an indicator of relative bite force in the former two species, but not in the latter. We argue that interspecific variation in territorial behaviour might explain this difference. Key-words: Honest signalling, lizards, sexual selection Functional Ecology (2005) Ecological Society Introduction Secondary sexual traits evolve through the process of sexual selection, either via female choice, male male competition or a combination of both (see Berglund, Bisazza & Pilastro 1996). In the context of male male competition, secondary sexual traits are used as signals, which may function as reliable cues for a male s overall robustness and/or fighting ability (Berglund et al. 1996). To determine whether a signal is honest with regard to a male s quality or social status, researchers typically test whether a link exists between the variation in the design (i.e. size, shape, coloration) of secondary sexual traits (e.g. antlers, plumage) and the variation in the ability to reproduce successfully and/or win fights with other males (e.g. Backwell & Passmore 1996; Jennions & Backwell 1996; Candolin 1999; Backwell et al. 2000). Relatively few studies, however, have measured whether the size or shape of a secondary sexual structure is related to ecologically relevant performance capacities, particularly those that are directly related to fighting Author to whom correspondence should be addressed. *Present address: Department of Biology, University of Antwerp, Universiteitsplein 1, B-2610 Wilrijk, Belgium. E-mail: bieke.vanhooydonck@ua.ac.be ability (see Brandt 2003; Perry et al. 2004). This is surprising since studies of performance (e.g. bite performance, locomotor endurance) offer an excellent opportunity for testing whether male signals are honest indicators of a male s quality, as measured by their ability to perform ecologically relevant tasks. Moreover, rendering inferences between signal design and performance are an excellent complement to typical male male combat studies. In this study, we examine variation in a secondary sexual trait, i.e. dewlap size, in three species of Jamaican Anolis lizards. Anolis lizards are characterized by the possession of an extensible throat fan, or dewlap. There is extensive variation in dewlap size and colour among and within Anolis species (Echelle, Echelle & Fitch 1978; Fitch & Hillis 1984; Losos & Chu 1998; Jenssen, Orrell & Lovern 2000) and dewlap expansions appear to be an important component of the anoline display behaviour in various contexts. In aggressive male male interactions, the dewlap is believed to represent a threat or challenge to potential intruders (Greenberg & Noble 1944; Jenssen et al. 2000); females, on the other hand, appear to choose males based on dewlap characteristics and are more receptive towards males that are performing dewlap extensions (Greenberg & Noble 1944; Crews 1975). Finally, in a predator prey context 38

39 Dewlap size and bite performance in Anolis the dewlap seems to act as a pursuit-deterrent signal as snakes stop moving towards dewlapping male anoles (Leal & Rodriguez-Robles 1997a,Leal & Rodriguez- Robles 1997b; Leal 1999). However, no studies have examined whether the relative size of the dewlap is an honest indicator of fighting ability, or one of its components (e.g. bite performance). Several lines of evidence indicate a strong link between relative (i.e. size-adjusted) bite force and male male fighting ability in lizards. First, Anolis lizard males will bite one another during fights, sometimes even slam one another against the substrate during physical combat (Greenberg & Noble 1944; McMann 1993; Stamps & Krishnan 1997, 1998; Jenssen et al. 2000). Thus, one would expect that individual anole males with relatively high bite forces would possess an advantage during male male contests relative to similarly sized anole males with relatively weak bite forces. Recent work verifies this assumption, showing that, during staged male male contests, large A. carolinensis males with relatively high bite forces won significantly more fights against similarly sized males with relatively weak bite forces (Lailvaux et al. 2004). Second, several studies have shown a direct link between relative head size (which is typically proportional to relative bite force in lizards; Herrel et al. 1999; Herrel et al. 2001) and the ability to win male male contests, or acquire high-quality territories (Hews 1990; Pratt et al. 1992; Molina-Borja et al. 1998; Gvozdik & Van Damme 2003; Perry et al. 2004). In short, a compelling body of evidence indicates that individual male lizards with enhanced biting abilities are at a competitive advantage for winning fights with other males. We measured bite force and dewlap size in a size series of adult males of A. lineatopus Gray 1840, A. grahami Gray 1845 and A. valencienni Duméril & Bibron 1837 at a single lowland scrub forest locality in north-eastern Jamaica. The three species differ dramatically in morphological shape (e.g. limb, tail dimensions; Losos 1990), display behaviour (e.g. percentage time spent displaying; Losos 1990; Irschick & Losos 1996) and territoriality (Hicks & Trivers 1983; Irschick & Losos 1996). Nevertheless, the three species are similarly sized, and syntopic, thus offering an excellent point of comparison. Within each species, we tested whether relative dewlap size accurately predicts relative bite force, thus constituting an honest signal. Materials and methods STUDY ANIMALS Adult individuals of three species of Anolis lizards were caught by noose or hand at the Discovery Bay Marine Laboratory (DBML), Discovery Bay, Jamaica, between 21 January 2002 and 2 February 2002. In total, we captured 11 adult male A. grahami, 31 adult male A. lineatopus and 13 adult male A. valencienni. Head length (HL) of all individuals was measured from the tip of the snout to the posterior edge of the parietal scale using digital callipers (Mitutoyo CD-15DC, Mitutoyo (UK) Ltd, Telford, UK; precision 0 01 mm). Measurements of bite force and dewlap size were completed at the DBML field station on the same or following day the lizards were caught; after experimentation, all lizards were released at the same site where they had been caught. MEASUREMENT OF BITE CAPACITY We measured bite force for all individuals using an isometric Kistler force transducer (type 9203) mounted on a purpose-built holder and connected to a Kistler charge amplifier (type 5995, Kistler Inc., Winterthur, Switzerland; see Herrel et al. 1999 for detailed description of set-up). We induced the lizards to bite the force transducer by tapping them softly on the side of the mouth. The tapping readily resulted in a characteristic threat response where the jaws are opened maximally. The free ends of the holder, i.e. the bite plates, were then placed between the jaws, which immediately resulted in fierce and prolonged biting. As body temperature is known to affect performance capacity in lizards, we placed the lizards outside (in the shade) in individual bags at least half an hour prior to experimentation and in between trials. In doing so, the lizards attained body temperatures equal or close to the environmental temperature, which approximated 30 C. Because these three species occur sympatrically, and do not clearly differ in their microclimatic preferences (Schoener & Schoener 1971), it is reasonable to examine bite force at the same ambient temperatures for each species. We scored each bite as good or poor. Poor trials were eliminated from the analyses. As an estimate of maximal bite capacity, we used the highest bite force out of the five bites for each individual. MEASUREMENT OF DEWLAP SIZE To measure dewlap size, we positioned the lizard sideways and pulled the base of the ceratobranchial, near its articulation with the basihyoid, gently forward with a pair of forceps. Since a dewlap consists of a skin flap attached to the lizard s throat on the one side and to the hyoid bone on the other, the dewlap becomes maximally extended when the ceratobranchial is pulled forward completely. We then positioned the lizard in such a manner that the extended dewlap was laid flat on a piece of paper. We traced the outer edge of the dewlap with a pencil (scale 1:1). Afterwards, the drawings were digitized using the Scion Image for Windows software (v4 0.2; Scion Corporation 2000, Frederick, MD) and dewlap size was quantified for each individual. To determine whether this method yields a reliable estimate of dewlap size, we drew and digitized the dewlaps of five individuals twice and calculated the repeatability of our measurement (Lessells & Boag 1987). Repeatability proved to be very high (r = 0 90, F 1,4 = 44 67, P < 0 0001).

40 B. Vanhooydonck et al. ANALYSES All data on HL, bite force and dewlap size were logarithmically (log 10 ) transformed prior to statistical analyses. All statistical analyses were done using SPSS v.11 5 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). To test whether absolute dewlap size is a good predictor of absolute bite force, we regressed log 10 bite force against log 10 dewlap size using ordinary linear leastsquares regression. To remove the effects of overall size on the dewlap size bite force relationship, we regressed log 10 bite force and log 10 dewlap size against log 10 HL and calculated the residual values for all individuals. We then regressed residual bite force (dependent variable) against residual dewlap size (independent variable). All of these regression analyses were done within each species. Results The regression of absolute bite force vs absolute dewlap size was significant within all three Anolis species (A. grahami: r = 0 93, F 1,9 = 55 66, P < 0 0001; A. lineatopus: r = 0 90; F 1,29 = 126 05, P < 0 0001; A. valencienni: r = 0 87, F 1,11 = 35 01, P < 0 0001). Thus, absolute dewlap size is a good predictor of absolute bite force in all three species. The regression of relative bite force against relative dewlap size, on the other hand, was significant within A. lineatopus (r = 0 47, F 1,29 = 8 39, P = 0 007; Fig. 1a) and A. grahami (r = 0 80, F 1,9 = 16 28, P = 0 003; Fig. 1b). The former significant relationships suggest that for a given body size, males with larger dewlaps bite harder in A. lineatopus and A. grahami. The regression of relative bite force against relative dewlap size was clearly non-significant within the remaining species, A. valencienni (r = 0 08, F 1,11 = 0 07, P = 0 79; Fig. 1c). Thus, relative dewlap size is not a good predictor of relative bite capacity within A. valencienni. Discussion PERFORMANCE AND THE EVOLUTION OF HONEST SIGNALLING Our results show that absolute dewlap size is a good predictor of absolute bite force in all three Jamaican species. After removing the effects of body size, however, the correlation between dewlap size and bite force remained significant within A. lineatopus, and A. grahami. Thus, in A. lineatopus and in A. grahami, males with large dewlaps, for their body size, tend to bite relatively hard. Thus, our results provide evidence that within at least two species of Anolis relative dewlap size is an honest predictor of an individual s fighting capacity as indicated by bite performance. In a similar fashion, display rate appears to indicate endurance capacity in other lizard species, i.e. Uta stansburiana (Brandt 2003) and A. cristatellus (Perry et al. 2004). Fig. 1. Residual bite force vs residual dewlap size for three Anolis species: (a) residual bite force is positively correlated to residual dewlap size in A. lineatopus (r = 0 47, P = 0 007); (b) residual bite force is significantly correlated to residual dewlap size in A. grahami (r = 0 80, P = 0 003); (c) residual bite force is not correlated to residual dewlap size in A. valencienni (r = 0 08, P = 0 79). By contrast, within the twig anole A. valencienni there was no apparent relationship between relative dewlap size and relative bite force. The fact that the dewlap seems to represent a signal in A. lineatopus and A. grahami, but not in A. valencienni might be explained by differences in the degree of territoriality. Both A. lineatopus and A. grahami are territorial anoles (Schoener & Schoener 1971). Territorial male anoles are known to aggressively defend their territories from intruders by using visual displays, including dewlap extensions. Thus, our data suggest that A. lineatopus and A. grahami males that bite relatively hard will also enjoy enhanced success in male male encounters, although future studies

41 Dewlap size and bite performance in Anolis that test this possibility explicitly would be useful. If true, then bite force should be subject to sexual selection via male male competition. On the other extreme, A. valencienni, a twig anole, is far less territorial (Hicks & Trivers 1983; Irschick & Losos 1996). This species is highly arboreal and cryptic, and males display less frequently (Irschick & Losos 1996). Further, male A. valencienni overlap in their home ranges substantially (Hicks & Trivers 1983). Anolis valencienni males therefore might not rely on biting during male male encounters to the same extent as A. lineatopus and A. grahami. Thus, a coupling between dewlap size and bite capacity may not have evolved within A. valencienni, because males of this species may not engage in physical combat to the same extent as A. lineatopus and A. grahami. It is important to note, however, that the dewlap is a multipurpose signal structure that is used for displaying to rival males, but also to potential mates, and predators (Leal & Rodríguez-Robles 1997a,b; Leal 1999). For example, detailed studies with the Puerto Rican anoles A. cuvieri and A. cristatellus show that during staged encounters with a model snake predator, male anoles displayed their dewlaps vigorously, and such vigorous displays appeared to be effective in deterring some attacks. These studies also reveal, however, that bite force could be important as a defensive mechanism against predators, as anoles would often bite and struggle when captured by snakes (Leal & Rodríguez-Robles 1995). Thus, the size of the dewlap may also have evolved as a signal to warn predators that anoles have powerful bites. Consequently, dewlap size in Anolis lizards has likely evolved in response to multiple ecological pressures (female attraction, male male competition, and antipredator signalling; Greenberg & Noble 1944; Crews 1975; Leal & Rodríguez-Robles 1997a,b; Losos & Chu 1998; Leal 1999; Jenssen et al. 2000). Even though we found a significant correlation between dewlap size and bite force, at least in some species, we note that the role of dewlap size in sexual selection is still not well understood. In A. sagrei, for instance, males do not display differently from conspecific males with or without (surgically removed) dewlaps, suggesting limited functional significance of the display of a male s dewlap during male male interactions (Tokarz, Paterson & McMann 2003). While these results do not give information about the role of the dewlap in other contexts (e.g. predator encounters, female attraction; see above), it is true that dewlap traits besides size (e.g. colour, shape, movement) and different components of display behaviour (e.g. display rate, display repertoire) might (also) be indicators of performance in Anolis lizards. Similarly, factors other than relative bite force may also be important for predicting whether male lizards will win or lose territorial fights. For example, previous studies have shown that locomotor performance is also an important determinant of male dominance in lizards (Garland, Hankins & Huey 1990; Robson & Miles 2000; Perry et al. 2004). Indeed, while we argue that relative bite force is an important aspect of male fighting capacity in lizards, we do not exclude other possibly covarying factors. Conclusions We show that the size of a secondary sexual trait (dewlap size) is linked with a relevant aspect of fighting capacity (bite force) in territorial Anolis species, but not in non-territorial anoles. Our results suggest that dewlap size is sexually selected for through male male territorial competition, and might serve as a visual cue to a male s fighting ability. It would be interesting to study whether similar relationships between secondary sexual traits (e.g. crests, horns, throat and body coloration, colour patches) and performance exist in other lizard groups. Acknowledgements This work was supported by an NSF grant (IBN 9983003) to Duncan Irschick. Bieke Vanhooydonck and Anthony Herrel are postdoctoral fellows at the Fund for Scientific Research Flanders (FWO Vl). We would like to thank the anonymous reviewer and Charles Fox for constructive comments on a previous draft of this paper. References Backwell, P.R.Y. & Passmore, N.I. (1996) Time constraints and multiple choice criteria in the sampling behaviour and mate choice of the fiddler crab, Uca annulipes. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 38, 407 416. Backwell, P.R.Y., Christy, J.H., Telford, S.R., Jennions, M.D. & Passmore, N.I. (2000) Dishonest signaling in a fiddler crab. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 267, 719 724. Berglund, A., Bisazza, A. & Pilastro, A. (1996) Armaments and ornaments: an evolutionary explanation of traits of dual utility. Biology Journal of the Linnean Society 58, 385 399. Brandt, Y. (2003) Lizard threat display handicaps endurance. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 270, 1061 1068. Candolin, U. (1999) Male male competition facilitates female choice in sticklebacks. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 266, 785 789. Crews, D. (1975) Effects of different components of male courtship behaviour on environmentally induced ovarian recrudescence and mating preferences in the lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Animal Behaviour 23, 349 356. Echelle, A.F., Echelle, A.A. & Fitch, H.S. (1978) Inter-and intraspecific allometry in a display organ: the dewlap of Anolis (Iguanidae) species. Copeia 1978, 245 250. Fitch, H.S. & Hillis, D.M. (1984) The Anolis dewlap: interspecific variability and morphological associations with habitat. Copeia 1984, 315 325. Garland, T. Jr, Hankins, E. & Huey, R.B. (1990) Locomotor capacity and social dominance in male lizards. Functional Ecology 4, 243 250. Greenberg, B. & Noble, G.K. (1944) Social behavior of the American chameleon (Anolis carolinensis Voigt). Physiological Zoology 17, 392 439. Gvozdik, L. & Van Damme, R. (2003) Evolutionary maintenance of sexual dimorphism in head size in the lizard Zootoca vivipara: a test of two hypotheses. Journal of Zoology, London 259, 7 13.

42 B. Vanhooydonck et al. Herrel, A., Spithoven, L., Van Damme, R. & De Vree, F. (1999) Sexual dimorphism of head size in Gallotia galloti: testing the niche divergence hypothesis by functional analysis. Functional Ecology 13, 289 297. Herrel, A., De Grauw, E. & Lemos-Espinal, J.A. (2001) Head shape and bite force in xenosaurid lizards. Journal of Experimental Zoology 290, 101 107. Hews, D. (1990) Examining hypotheses generated by field measures of sexual selection on male lizards, Uta palmeri. Evolution 44, 1956 1966. Hicks, R.A. & Trivers, R.L. (1983) The social behavior of Anolis valencienni. Advances in Herpetology and Evolutionary Biology: Essays in Honor of Ernest E. Williams (eds A.G.J. Rhodin & K. Miyata), pp. 570 595. Museum of Comparative Zoology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA. Irschick, D.J. & Losos, J.B. (1996) Morphology, ecology, and behavior of the twig anole Anolis angusticeps. Contributions to West Indian Herpetology: A Tribute to Albert Schwartz (eds B. Powell & B. Henderson), pp. 291 301. Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles, New York. Jennions, M.D. & Backwell, P.R.Y. (1996) Residency and size affect fight duration and outcome in the fiddler crab Uca annulipes. Biology Journal of the Linnean Society 57, 293 306. Jenssen, T.A., Orrell, K.S. & Lovern, M.B. (2000) Sexual dimorphisms in aggressive signal structure and use by a polygynous lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Copeia 2000, 140 149. Lailvaux, S., Herrel, A., Vanhooydonck, B., Meyers, J. & Irschick, D.J. (2004) Performance capacity, fighting tactics, and the evolution of life-stage morphs in the green anole lizard (Anolis carolinensis). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B 271, 2501 2508. Leal, M. (1999) Honest signalling during prey predator interactions in the lizard Anolis cristatellus. Animal Behaviour 58, 521 526. Leal, M. & Rodríguez-Robles, J.A. (1995) Antipredator responses of Anolis cristatellus (Sauria: Polychrotidae). Copeia 1995, 155 161. Leal, M. & Rodríguez-Robles, J.A. (1997a) Antipredator responses of the Puerto Rican anole Anolis cuvieri (Squamata: Polychrotidae). Biotropica 29, 372 375. Leal, M. & Rodríguez-Robles, J.A. (1997b) Signalling displays during predator prey interactions in a Puerto Rican anole, Anolis cristatellus. Animal Behaviour 54, 1147 1154. Lessells, C.M. & Boag, P.T. (1987) Unrepeatable repeatabilities: a common mistake. Auk 104, 116 121. Losos, J.B. (1990) Concordant evolution of locomotor behaviour, display rate and morphology in Anolis lizards. Animal Behaviour 39, 879 890. Losos, J.B. & Chu, L. (1998) Examination of factors potentially affecting dewlap size in Caribbean anoles. Copeia 1998, 430 438. McMann, S. (1993) Contextual signalling and the structure of dyadic encounters in Anolis carolinensis. Animal Behaviour 46, 657 668. Molina-Borja, M., Padron-Fumero, M. & Alfonso-Martin, T. (1998) Morphological and behavioural traits affecting the intensity and outcome of male contests in Gallotia galloti galloti (Family Lacertidae). Ethology 104, 314 322. Perry, G., Levering, K., Girard, I. & Garland, T. Jr (2004) Locomotor performance and dominance in male Anolis cristatellus. Animal Behaviour 67, 37 47. Pratt, N.C., Alberts, A.C., Fulton-Medler, K.G. & Phillips, J.A. (1992) Behavioral, physiological, and morphological components of dominance and mate attraction in male green iguanas. Zoo Biology 11, 153 163. Robson, M.A. & Miles, D.B. (2000) Locomotor performance and dominance in tree lizards, Urosaurus ornatus. Functional Ecology 14, 338 344. Schoener, T.W. & Schoener, A. (1971) Structural habitats of West Indian Anolis lizards I. Lowland Jamaica. Breviora 368, 1 53. Stamps, J.A. & Krishnan, V.V. (1997) Functions of fights in territory establishment. American Naturalist 150, 393 405. Stamps, J.A. & Krishnan, V.V. (1998) Territory acquisition in lizards. IV. Obtaining high status and exclusive home ranges. Animal Behaviour 55, 461 472. Tokarz, R.R., Paterson, A.V. & McMann, S. (2003) Laboratory and field test of the functional significance of the male s dewlap in the lizard Anolis sagrei. Copeia 2003, 502 511. Received 29 April 2004; revised 27 September 2004; accepted 6 October 2004