Pair formation among experimentally introduced mallards Anas platyrhynchos reflects habitat quality

Similar documents
Anas clypeata (Northern Shoveler)

The Influence of Food Abundance Patterns and Predation on Breeding Ducks with a Special Emphasis on the Interactions between Ducklings and Fish

GENERAL NOTES 675. Reproductive behavior and pairing chronology in wintering dabbling ducks.-

DO BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS?

Reduced availability of refuse and breeding output in a herring gull (Larus argentatus) colony

TIME BUDGET OF BREEDING NORTHERN SHOVELERS

Why are there so many empty lakes? Food limits survival of mallard ducklings

Woodcock: Your Essential Brief

Population Study of Canada Geese of Jackson Hole

Notes and Discussion

MDWFP Aerial Waterfowl Survey Report. January 8-11, 2019

Population dynamics of small game. Pekka Helle Natural Resources Institute Finland Luke Oulu

PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE

NORMAN R. SEYMOUR & SEAN C. MITCHELL 1. Abstract

The fall and the rise of the Swedish Peregrine Falcon population. Peter Lindberg

Alien egg retrieval in common pochard: Do females discriminate between conspecific and heterospecific eggs?

Biodiversity and Extinction. Lecture 9

FREQUENCY AND TIMING OF SECOND BROODS IN WOOD DUCKS

Postnatal effects of incubation length in mallard and pheasant chicks

Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey

Waterfowl Population Status, 2001

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

Living Planet Report 2018

October 1980] Short Communications 875

The female Mallard s call is a loud quack-quack similar to that given by farmyard ducks. The call of the male is a softer, low-pitched rhab-rhab.

Unit E: Other Poultry. Lesson 2: Exploring the Duck Industry

Experimental evidence for a mismatch between insect emergence and waterfowl hatching under increased spring temperatures

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

BROOD PARASITISM AMONG WATERFOWL NESTING ON ISLANDS AND PENINSULAS IN NORTH DAKOTA

Puddle Ducks Order Anseriformes Family Anatinae Subfamily Anatini

MDWFP Aerial Waterfowl Survey Report. December 11-13, 2017

TIME OF PAIRING OF AMERICAN

Mallard and Blue-winged Teal Philopatry in Northwest Wisconsin

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp

PORTRAIT OF THE AMERICAN BALD EAGLE

206 Adopted: 4 April 1984

Local Grains and Free-Choice Feeding of Organic Layer Hens on Pasture at UBC Farm Introduction

Habitat Report. May 21, 2013

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Gruiformes Family: Cariamidae Scientific Name: Cariama cristata Common Name: Red-legged seriema

Sexy smells Featured scientist: Danielle Whittaker from Michigan State University

A POSSIBLE FACTOR IN THE EVOLUTION OF CLUTCH SIZE IN ROSS GOOSE JOHN P. RYDER

FREE-LIVING WILLOW PTARMIGAN ARE DETERMINATE EGG-LAYERS

The Influence of Diet Quality on Clutch Size and Laying Pattern in Mallards

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

SEASONAL PATTERNS OF NESTING IN THE RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD MORTALITY

rodent species in Australia to the fecal odor of various predators. Rattus fuscipes (bush

Date submitted to OIE 09/03/2017 LONDON SW1P 3JR

08 alberts part2 7/23/03 9:10 AM Page 95 PART TWO. Behavior and Ecology

Quantifying density dependence in a bird population using human disturbance

Getting started with adaptive management of migratory waterbirds in Europe: The challenge of multifaceted interests

Habitat Report. July 2011

From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research. Animal behavior then & now

THE JAPANESE CRANE. endangered species L ARCHE PHOTOGRAPHIQUE CHARACTERISTICS

Experimental evidence for density-dependent survival in mallard (Anas platyrhynchos) ducklings

Avian Ecology: Life History, Breeding Seasons, & Territories

FOOD HABITS OF NESTING COOPER S HAWKS AND GOSHAWKS IN NEW YORK AND PENNSYLVANIA

Estelar CHAPTER-6 RAISING AND PRODUCTION OF POULTRY BIRDS

Ministry of Agriculture. HPAI in Hungary

Weights of wild Mallard Anas platyrhynchos, Gadwall A. streperà, and Blue-winged Teal A. discors during the breeding season

REGIONAL VARIATION IN COWBIRD PARASITISM OF WOOD THRUSHES

Habitat Report. Sept 2012

THE STATUS OF TRUMPETER SWANS IN NEW YORK STATE IN 2007

Sex ratios in some common British wintering ducks

Local Conservation Action leads to Breeding Success for Critically Endangered BAER S POCHARD at Hengshui Hu.

AS91603 Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants & animals to their external environment

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns

RECESSIVE BUDGIES: A BEGINNERS INTRODUCTION TO RECESSIVES IN BUDGERIGARS.

BIOLOGY 436: WATERFOWL BIOLOGY AND WETLAND MANAGEMENT COURSE SYLLABUS

SPACING PATTERNS, MATING SYSTEMS, AND WINTER PHILOPATRY IN HARLEQUIN DUCKS

VANCOUVER ISLAND MARMOT

Current Status of Amphibian Populations. Amphibian biology - characteristics making

Date submitted to OIE 16/03/2018 LONDON SW1P 3JR

Subject: Preliminary Draft Technical Memorandum Number Silver Lake Waterfowl Survey

A "New" Allele of the Mallard Plumage Pattern in Ducks

MDWFP Aerial Waterfowl Survey Report. January 19 and 24-25, 2018

VANCOUVER ISLAND MARMOT

ESIA Albania Annex 11.4 Sensitivity Criteria

NEST PROSPECTING BY COMMON GOLDENEYES

The story of Solo the Turnbull National Wildlife Refuge Male Swan

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

Great Blue Heron Chick Development. Through the Stages

Get Instant Access to ebook Keeping Ducks PDF at Our Huge Library KEEPING DUCKS PDF. ==> Download: KEEPING DUCKS PDF

Key concepts of Article 7(4): Version 2008

Ducks of Florida 1. Dabbling Ducks WEC243. Emma Willcox and William Giuliano 2

WATTLED CRANE RECOVERY PROGRAMME Ensuring that Wattled Cranes continue to grace the skies and wetlands of South Africa

Conflict and cooperation: a really short guide to the family life of birds

EIDER JOURNEY It s Summer Time for Eiders On the Breeding Ground

WETLAND SELECTION BY MALLARDS AND BLUE-WINGED TEAL

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission.

PARK WATERFOWL POPULATIONS IN MASSACHUSETTS. By H W HEUSMANN AND RICHARD BURRELL

Subfamily Anserinae. Waterfowl Identification WFS 340. Mute Swan. Order Anseriformes. Family Anatidae

Quack FAQs: Is there a Mother Duck on your Roof? Has a mother duck built her nest on your balcony or roof -- or in your courtyard?

Solenopsis geminata (Tropical Fire Ant)

D u ck num bers in the USSR, the Western Palearctic and North America : first comparisons

Introduction. Description. This duck

INTER-FAMILY DOMINANCE IN CANADA GEESE

During courting, the male utters a moaning, almost dove-like, ik-ik-cooo cry. The female answers with a low quacking cuk-cuk.

Fall and Spring Body Weights and Condition Indices of Ducks in Illinois

RESTORATION OF A DECLINING POPULATION OF PEREGRINE FALCONS IN SWEDEN THROUGH CAPTIVE BREEDING: 30 YEARS OF EXPERIENCE

Transcription:

Ann. Zool. Fennici 38: 179 184 ISSN 0003-455X Helsinki 26 June 2001 Finnish Zoological and Botanical Publishing Board 2001 Pair formation among experimentally introduced mallards Anas platyrhynchos reflects habitat quality Hannu Pöysä 1 *, Kjell Sjöberg 2, Johan Elmberg 2# & Petri Nummi 3 1) Finnish Game and Fisheries Research Institute, Evo Game Research Station, Kaitalammintie 75, FIN-16970 Evo, Finland (e-mail: hannu.poysa@rktl.fi) * Present address: Finnish Game and Fisheries Research Institute, Joensuu Game and Fisheries Research, Kauppakatu 18 20, FIN-80100 Joensuu, Finland 2) Department of Animal Ecology, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, SE-901 83 Umeå, Sweden # Present address: Department of Mathematics and Natural Sciences, Kristianstad University, SE-29188 Kristianstad, Sweden 3) Department of Applied Zoology, University of Helsinki, P.O. Box 27 (Viikki C), FIN-00014 University of Helsinki, Finland Received 14 December 2000, accepted 7 March 2001 Pöysä, H., Sjöberg, K., Elmberg, J. & Nummi, P. 2001: Pair formation among experimentally introduced mallards Anas platyrhynchos reflects habitat quality. Ann. Zool. Fennici 38: 179 184. Using data from two independent field experiments, we address whether pair formation in introduced mallards Anas platyrhynchos is associated with habitat quality, specifically food limitation at the brood stage. Based on the concentration of total phosphorous in the water, the study lakes were divided into two groups, poor and rich. In one of the experiments we used mallard ducklings imprinted on humans to study mass change of ducklings in poor and rich lakes, respectively. It turned out that ducklings foraging on poor lakes gained less mass than ducklings foraging on rich lakes, the division of lakes thus reflecting habitat quality at the brood stage. Introduced mallards formed heterosexual pairs on lakes that were, in a relative sense, high-quality brood habitats, whereas they did not on lakes of low-quality brood habitat. Pair formation thus seemed to reflect the suitability of habitat for breeding.

180 Pöysä et al. ANN. ZOOL. FENNICI Vol. 38 Introduction Food shortage is an important aspect of habitat quality influencing life history traits (Martin 1987) and several population and community level processes (Wiens 1989, Boutin 1990, Newton 1998). In a series of experimental studies, we addressed the role of food in habitat selection and distribution of breeding mallards Anas platyrhynchos (Pöysä et al. 1998, 2000, Sjöberg et al. 2000). In an introduction experiment, we tested the hypotheses of conspecific attraction (Stamps 1988) and ideal preemption (Pulliam & Danielson 1991) in habitat selection of nesting mallards (Pöysä et al. 1998; see also Elmberg et al. 1997). We found that in some lakes introduced birds attracted wild conspecifics arriving from spring migration, whereas in other lakes they did not. In another field experiment focusing on habitat quality, specifically food limitation at the brood stage, we found that some lakes were inferior brood habitats and that habitat quality at the brood stage may affect the distribution of nesting pairs (Sjöberg et al. 2000). We also found that, independent of treatment, the outcome of the introduction experiment was at least partially dependent on the quality of the lakes as brood-rearing habitats (Pöysä et al. 2000). Hence, habitat quality seems important to processes at individual as well as at population level in breeding mallards. In the introduction experiment, we observed variation in pair formation among introduced mallards. On some lakes, they split up into pairs, whereas on other lakes they did not. In this paper, we focus on this variation. By using data from two independent field experiments, we ask whether pair formation among introduced birds is associated with habitat quality, specifically food limitation at the brood stage. Material and methods This analysis is based on data gathered in two independent field experiments that have been reported in full elsewhere (Pöysä et al. 1998, Sjöberg et al. 2000). Both were done in the Umeå area, central Sweden (about 63 30 N, 20 E; map given in Elmberg et al. 1993: fig. 1), an area situated in the mid-boreal zone, and being rich in lakes and wetlands. Lakes are icecovered in winter and the selection of nesting lakes is repeated each spring by migratory mallards. As details of the experimental procedures can be found in the original articles we here report on the most pertinent parts only. In a two-year introduction experiment (Elmberg et al. 1997, Pöysä et al. 1998), we used captive-bred, pen-raised and wing-clipped mallards obtained from the Swedish Sportsmen s Association from two breeders in Södermanland, south-central Sweden. The ducks arrived at Umeå in March, 6 8 weeks prior to release so that they were able to adjust to the local light regime. All birds were treated identically, and they were kept in one big group in an unheated indoor pen and fed a combination of poultry pellets, whole barley and vitamins ad libitum. Using a cross-over design, we introduced six individuals (3 males and 3 females) randomly selected from the big group to each lake in spring just before the arrival of migrating wild mallards, i.e. between 2 and 6 May 1993 (16 lakes) and 1 and 4 May 1994 (16 other lakes) (Elmberg et al. 1997, Pöysä et al. 1998). Each lake received introduced mallards in one year only, the other year serving as a control in the introduction experiment (Pöysä et al. 1998; see also Elmberg et al. 1997). In both years, we censused introduced as well as wild mallards on the lakes. Two censuses were done in both years in the early part of the breeding season (8 21 May). Pair formation among the introduced birds was monitored and the number of separate pairs was recorded in each census. Observers were not aware of the lake classification (see next paragraph) at the time of censuses. Heterosexually paired birds close together, keeping a distance to other mallards, introduced or wild, were used as a qualitative definition of a pair; in practice, pair formation was obvious in the field. Sustained proximity to a member of the opposite sex has been used as a criterion to determine pair status also in other studies (e.g. Wishart 1983, Hepp & Hair 1984, Johnson & Rohwer 1998). We used the mean of the two censuses for the total number of introduced birds, the number of separate pairs, and the proportion of

ANN. ZOOL. FENNICI Vol. 38 Pair formation and habitat quality 181 birds in pairs as dependent variables. In the other experiment conducted in June 1996, we used mallard ducklings imprinted on humans to address brood-stage food limitation by studying mass change of ducklings (Sjöberg et al. 2000). Based on the concentration of total phosphorous in the water, the study lakes were divided into two groups; poor and rich. Total phosphorous is a reasonably good indicator of the trophic status of lakes (Kerekes et al. 1990, Staicer et al. 1994, Jeppesen et al. 2000). From a group of 31 four-day-old mallard ducklings we randomly selected 12 to always be used in poorlake foraging trials and another 12 to always be used in rich-lake foraging trials, the seven remaining ducklings being used as controls. All 31 ducklings were kept in a pen with free access to food when not in experimental trials. On each experiment day the food was taken away at 8 a.m. and the 12 ducklings of both experiment groups were collected for daytime foraging trials, the 7 control ducklings remaining in the pen with access to food. One trial was run per lake and parallel trials were run on the same days on a poor lake (n = 11) and a rich lake (n = 11). Each trial lasted 6 hours, usually starting at 10 a.m., and each duckling, including the controls, was weighed before and after the trial. After trials the ducklings which had been foraging on lakes were brought back to the pen, and all 31 ducklings were kept together there with free access to food until the beginning of the next day s trial. It turned out that ducklings foraging on poor lakes gained significantly less mass than ducklings foraging on rich lakes (Sjöberg et al. 2000). Poor lakes were thus clearly inferior to rich lakes as brood habitat. For the present analysis we used data from lakes used in both of the aforementioned experiments. In all, we had 14 such lakes, seven of them being poor lakes and the other seven rich lakes according to Sjöberg et al. (2000). Because introduced birds disappeared from one poor lake and from one rich lake (see also Elmberg et al. 1997, Pöysä et al. 1998), the final sample size was six for both lake types. We used the Mann-Whitney U-test in comparisons between poor and rich lakes. Analyses were run with SYSTAT procedures (Wilkinson 1992). Probability values are two-tailed. Fig. 1. Means (+ 1 SD) of (a) total number of individuals, (b) number of pairs, and (c) proportion of paired individuals of introduced mallards in poor lakes (n = 6) and rich lakes (n = 6), respectively. Six mallards (3 males, 3 females) were introduced to each lake; hence maximum values were six for number of individuals and three for pairs. See material and methods for further explanation. Results The mean total number of observed introduced mallards per census did not differ between poor and rich lakes (Fig. 1a; U = 22.5, p = 0.92). By

182 Pöysä et al. ANN. ZOOL. FENNICI Vol. 38 contrast, the mean number of pairs and, consequently, the proportion of paired individuals were higher on rich lakes than on poor (Fig. 1b and c; number of pairs, U = 35.5, p = 0.008; proportion of paired individuals, U = 35.5, p = 0.008). Generally speaking, introduced mallards formed separate pairs on lakes that were, in a relative sense, high-quality brood habitats, whereas they did not on lakes of low-quality brood habitat. Discussion Because pair formation of wild mallards normally takes place on wintering grounds (McKinney 1986, Bluhm 1988), it is clear that the pair formation of the introduced birds in our experiment does not fully reflect a natural situation. Nor do we claim that courtship and mate choice (e.g. Holmberg et al. 1989) in our wing-clipped birds in all details followed their normal behavioural routes. However, because renesting is frequent in mallards, pair formation involving mate switching also occurs in the breeding season (McKinney 1986 and references therein). Furthermore, unpaired males may frequently try to form pair-ponds during the breeding season (McKinney et al. 1983, Goodburn 1984). Therefore, we consider the pair formation among the introduced birds as a biologically reasonable measure of mallards preparing for breeding. Our results give further support for the role of habitat quality, especially food limitation at the brood stage, in individual and population level processes in breeding mallards. The absence of pair formation among the introduced birds on poor lakes suggests that, if food requirements at the brood stage are not met, a lake may not induce visiting birds to prepare for nesting, but it will rather remain without nesting pairs. We showed previously that the food resource level at the time of selecting a nesting lake can be used to predict the resource level at the brood stage (Pöysä et al. 2000). The present results agree with our earlier finding that a considerable proportion of seemingly suitable lakes on a landscape may remain unoccupied by nesting individuals (Pöysä et al. 1998, 2000, Sjöberg et al. 2000). Our present and earlier results for mallards have direct ties to the current approach of using individual behaviour in studying population level phenomena and conservation problems (e.g. O Connor 1985, Sutherland 1996, Fryxell & Lundberg 1997, Caro 1998, Sutherland 1998a, Reed 1999). This approach is especially timely and relevant for studies dealing with habitatrelated problems, because habitat deterioration and loss are considered to be major global threats to migratory bird populations, including Anatidae (e.g. Green 1996, Sutherland 1998b). The black duck Anas rubripes, ecologically very similar to the mallard, is one good example (Dwyer & Baldassarre 1994). We suggest that behavioural and other individual level aspects studied by us with respect to habitat quality, especially pair formation among introduced birds (this study), as well as response of imprinted ducklings to habitat quality (Nummi et al. 2000, Sjöberg et al. 2000; see also Pöysä et al. 2000) provide a tool for assessing the quality of wetland habitats. At this point it may appear tempting to use a surrogate, like the concentration of total P in the water, as a convenient shortcut to measure a lake s suitability as a brood rearing habitat (Kerekes et al. 1990, Staicer et al. 1994, Sjöberg et al. 2000). However, we advise against this. Although lakes with mallard pairs and broods generally have relatively high P concentrations, the opposite may not always be the case. That is, high productivity may manifest itself in high fish biomass rather than ducks. Therefore, using the mallards themselves to measure habitat quality should be a more reliable method. For example, our experimental procedures could be used in assessing a priori the suitability of man-made and natural habitats for the management of mallard and other wildfowl populations (Street 1977, The Game Conservancy 1993). In this way wildfowl release and habitat restoration programmes (e.g. Myrberget 1990, The Game Conservancy 1993, Callaghan & Kirby 1996) could be directed to profitable sites and wasting of limited resources avoided.

ANN. ZOOL. FENNICI Vol. 38 Pair formation and habitat quality 183 References Bluhm, C. 1988: Temporal patterns of pair formation and reproduction in annual cycles and associated endocrinology in waterfowl. In: Johnston, R. F. (ed.), Current ornithology, Vol 5: 123 185. Plenum Press, New York. Boutin, S. A. 1990: Food supplementation experiments with terrestrial vertebrates: patterns, problems, and the future. Can. J. Zool. 68: 203 220. Callaghan, D. A. & Kirby, J. S. 1996: Releases of Anatidae for hunting and the effects on wetland biodiversity: a review and evaluation. In: Birkan, M., van Vessem, J., Havet, P., Madsen, J. Trolliet, B. & Hainard, R. (eds.), Anatidae 2000. An International Conference on the Conservation, Habitat Management and Wise Use of Ducks, Geese and Swans: 547 555. Office National de la Chasse, Paris. Caro, T. (ed.) 1998: Behavioral ecology and conservation biology. Oxford Univ. Press, New York. Dwyer, C. P. & Baldassarre, G. A. 1994: Habitat use by sympatric female mallards and American black ducks breeding in a forested environment. Can. J. Zool. 72: 1538 1542. Elmberg, J., Nummi, P., Pöysä, H. & Sjöberg, K. 1993: Factors affecting species number and density of dabbling duck guilds in North Europe. Ecography 16: 251 260. Elmberg, J., Pöysä, H., Sjöberg, K. & Nummi, P. 1997: Interspecific interactions and co-existence in dabbling ducks: observations and an experiment. Oecologia 111: 129 136. Goodburn, S. F. 1984: Mate guarding in the mallard Anas platyrhynchos. Ornis Scand. 15: 261 265. Green, A. J. 1996: Analyses of globally threatened Anatidae in relation to threats, distribution, migration patterns, and habitat use. Conserv. Biol. 10: 1435 1445. Hepp, G. R. & Hair, J. D. 1984: Dominance in wintering waterfowl (Anatini): effects on distribution of sexes. Condor 86: 251 257. Holmberg, K., Edsman, L. & Klint, T. 1989: Female mate preferences and male attributes in mallard ducks Anas platyrhynchos. Anim. Behav. 39: 1 7. Jeppesen, E., Jensen, J. P., Søndergaard, M., Lauridsen, T. & Landkildehus, F. 2000: Trophic status, species richness and biodiversity in Danish lakes: changes along a phosphorous gradient. Freshw. Biol. 45: 201 208. Johnson, W. P. & Rohwer, F. C. 1998: Pairing chronology and agonistic behaviors of wintering green-winged teal and mallards. Wilson Bull. 110: 311 315. Kerekes, J. J., Blouin, A. C. & Beauchamp, S. T. 1990: Trophic response to phosporous in acidic and nonacidic lakes in Nova Scotia, Canada. Hydrobiologia 191: 105 110. Martin, T. E. 1987: Food as a limit on breeding birds: a life-history perspective. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 18: 453 487. McKinney, F. 1986: Ecological factors influencing the social systems of migratory dabbling ducks. In: Rubenstein, D. I. & Wrangham, R. W. (eds.), Ecological aspects of social evolution. Birds and mammals: 153 171. Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton. McKinney, F., Derrickson, S. R. & Mineau, P. 1983: Forced copulation in waterfowl. Behaviour 86: 250 294. Myrberget, S. 1990: Wildlife management in Europe outside the Soviet Union. NINA Utredning 18: 1 47. Newton, I. 1998: Population limitation in birds. Academic Press, San Diego. Nummi, P., Sjöberg, K., Pöysä, H. & Elmberg, J. 2000: Individual foraging behaviour indicates resource limitation: an experiment with mallard ducklings. Can. J. Zool. 78: 1891 1895. Pulliam, H. R. & Danielson, B. J. 1991: Sources, sinks, and habitat selection: a landscape perspective on population dynamics. Am. Nat. 137: S50 S66. Pöysä, H., Elmberg, J., Sjöberg, K. & Nummi, P. 1998: Habitat selection rules in breeding mallards (Anas platyrhynchos): a test of two competing hypotheses. Oecologia 114: 283 287. Pöysä, H., Elmberg, J., Sjöberg, K. & Nummi, P. 2000: Nesting mallards (Anas platyrhynchos) forecast brood stage food limitation when selecting habitat: experimental evidence. Oecologia 122: 582 586. Reed, J. M. 1999: The role of behavior in recent avian extinctions and endangerments. Conserv. Biol. 13: 232 241. Sjöberg, K., Pöysä, H., Elmberg, J. & Nummi, P. 2000: Response of mallard ducklings to variation in habitat quality: an experiment of food limitation. Ecology 81: 329 335. Staicer, C. A., Freedman, B., Srivastava, D., Dowd, N., Kilgar, J., Hayden, J., Payne, F. & Pollock, T. 1994: Use of lakes by black duck broods in relation to biological, chemical, and physical features. Hydrobiologia 279/280: 185 199. Stamps, J. A. 1988: Conspecific attraction and aggregation in territorial species. Am. Nat. 131: 329 347. Street, M. 1977: The food of mallard ducklings in a wet gravel quarry, and its relation to duckling survival. Wildfowl 28: 113 125. Sutherland, W. J. 1996: From individual behaviour to population ecology. Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford. Sutherland, W. J. 1998a: The importance of behavioural studies in conservation biology. Anim. Behav. 56: 801 809. Sutherland, W. J. 1998b: The effect of local change in habitat quality on populations of migratory species. J. Appl. Ecol. 35: 418 421. The Game Conservancy 1993: Ponds and lakes for

184 Pöysä et al. ANN. ZOOL. FENNICI Vol. 38 waterfowl. The Game Conservancy. Advisory Guide 3. The Game Conservancy, Hampshire. Wiens, J. A. 1989: The ecology of bird communities, Vol 1. Foundations and patterns. Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge. Wishart, R. A. 1983: Pairing chronology and mate selection in the American wigeon (Anas americana). Can. J. Zool. 61: 1733 1743. Wilkinson, L. 1992: SYSTAT: the system for statistics. SYSTAT, Evanston, Illinois.