HOMING BEHAVIOUR OF PIGEONS SUBJECTED TO UNILATERAL ZINC SULPHATE TREATMENT OF THEIR OLFACTORY MUCOSA

Similar documents
Homing in Pigeons: The Role of the Hippocampal Formation in the Representation of Landmarks Used for Navigation

RESEARCH ARTICLE Olfactory lateralization in homing pigeons: a GPS study on birds released with unilateral olfactory inputs

Pigeons with ablated pyriform cortex home from familiar but not from unfamiliar sites

The role of visual landmarks in the avian familiar area map

Effects of monocular viewing on orientation in an arena at the release site and homing performance in pigeons

PIGEONS AT MAGNETIC ANOMALIES: THE EFFECTS OF LOFT LOCATION BY CHARLES WALCOTT

METHODS FOR PRODUCING DISTURBANCES IN PIGEON HOMING BEHAVIOUR BY OSCILLATING MAGNETIC FIELDS

HOMING EXPERIMENTS WITH STARLINGS DEPRIVED OF THE SENSE OF SMELL

OLFACTORY CUES PERCEIVED AT THE HOME LOFT ARE NOT ESSENTIAL FOR THE FORMATION OF A NAVIGATIONAL MAP IN PIGEONS

Navigation-induced ZENK expression in the olfactory system of pigeons (Columba livia)

THE EFFECT ON PIGEON HOMING OF ANESTHESIA. CHARLES WALCOTT AND KLAUS SCHiYIIDT-KOENIG

Smelling home: a good solution for burrow-finding in nocturnal petrels?

COULD OSMOTAXIS EXPLAIN THE ABILITY OF BLUE PETRELS TO RETURN TO THEIR BURROWS AT NIGHT?

RESEARCH ARTICLE Evidence for discrete landmark use by pigeons during homing

How the viewing of familiar landscapes prior to release allows pigeons to home faster: evidence from GPS tracking

HOMING EXPERIMENTS ON PIGEONS SUBJECTED TO BILATERAL DESTRUCTION OF THE PARATYMPANIC ORGAN

The Effect of Phase Shifts in the Day-Night Cycle on Pigeon Homing at Distances of Less than One Mile

SUN-COMPASS ORIENTATION IN HOMING PIGEONS: COMPENSATION FOR DIFFERENT RATES OF CHANGE IN AZIMUTH?

RESEARCH ARTICLE A magnetic pulse does not affect homing pigeon navigation: a GPS tracking experiment

Animal Spatial Cognition:

RESEARCH ARTICLE Development of the navigational system in homing pigeons: increase in complexity of the navigational map

Hans G. Wallraff Avian Navigation: Pigeon Homing as a Paradigm

IN MEMORIAM: WILLIAM T. KEETON STEPHEN T. EMLEN. Section of Neurobiology and Behavior, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York USA

The influence of experience in orientation: GPS tracking of homing pigeons released over the sea after directional training

An edge-detection approach to investigating pigeon navigation

Testing the role of sensory systems in the migratory heading of a songbird

I. Introduction. Orientation and Navigation 3/8/2012. Most difficult problem Must know. How birds find their way. Two terms often misused

Hans G. Wallraff Avian Navigation: Pigeon Homing as a Paradigm

Avian Navigation: Pigeon Homing as a Paradigm

Zurich Open Repository and Archive. Flock flying improves pigeons' homing: GPS-track analysis of individual flyers versus small groups

JUDITH R. ALEXANDER AND WILLIAM T. KEETON

RESEARCH ARTICLE Oceanic navigation in Cory s shearwaters: evidence for a crucial role of olfactory cues for homing after displacement

The Development of Behavior

Publication list Peer-reviewed papers

ISSN , Volume 13, Number 3

Waved albatrosses can navigate with strong magnets attached to their head

Report. From Compromise to Leadership in Pigeon Homing

texp. Biol. (196a), 39,

This article is downloaded from.

Weaver Dunes, Minnesota

Altered Orientation and Flight Paths of Pigeons Reared on Gravity Anomalies: A GPS Tracking Study

INSTRUMENTATIONS TO INVESTIGATE MAGNETORECEPTION IN HOMING PIGEONS (COLUMBA LIVIA)

Exogenous chemical substances in bird perception: a review

RESEARCH ARTICLE Atmospheric propagation modeling indicates homing pigeons use loft-specific infrasonic ʻmapʼ cues

$? 479 THE FUNCTION OF M. DEPRESSOR CAUDAE AND M. CAUDOFEMORALIS IN PIGEONS

Navigation by green turtles: which strategy do displaced adults use to find Ascension Island?

AnOn. Behav., 1971, 19,

Sense of Smell. By: Liz, Gen, Ethan, and Meakena

Tuning a nose to forage: Evidence for olfactory learning in a procellariiform seabird chicks

PERCEPTION OF OCEAN WAVE DIRECTION BY SEA TURTLES

Orientation of lizards in a Morris water-maze: roles of the sun compass and the parietal eye

Station 1. Echolocation

Contributions of reproductive experience to observation-maintained crop growth and incubation in male and female ring doves

AN INVESTIGATION OF HOMING ABILITY IN PIGEONS WITHOUT PREVIOUS HOMING EXPERIENCE

Evidence for perceptual neglect of environmental features in hippocampal-lesioned pigeons during homing

Navigation and seasonal migratory orientation in juvenile sea turtles

Supplementary Fig. 1: Comparison of chase parameters for focal pack (a-f, n=1119) and for 4 dogs from 3 other packs (g-m, n=107).

Migration. Migration = a form of dispersal which involves movement away from and subsequent return to the same location, typically on an annual basis.

BEHAVIOUR OF DOGS DURING OLFACTORY TRACKING

Charles Walcott 2 December 2012

rodent species in Australia to the fecal odor of various predators. Rattus fuscipes (bush

This article is downloaded from.

Mysterious Death on the Greenway

SOCIOMETRIC INVESTIGATIONS IN GROUPS OF WILD AND DOMESTIC RABBITS WITH ONE BUCK AND TWO OR THREE DOES

THE STUDY OF HOW THE NOSE AFFECTS THE TASTE OF FOOD. Maddie Mizelle. Cary Academy ABSTRACT

HOMING OF SINGLE PIGEONS ANALYSIS OF TRACKS

Pre-natal construction of neural circuits (the highways are genetically specified):

Animal Navigation: Behavioral strategies and sensory cues

THE EFFECT OF DISTRACTERS ON STUDENT PERFORMANCE ON THE FORCE CONCEPT INVENTORY

DETECTION OF MAGNETIC INCLINATION ANGLE BY SEA TURTLES: A POSSIBLE MECHANISM FOR DETERMINING LATITUDE

Directional tracking in the domestic dog, Canis familiaris

SOME OBSERVATIONS ON PECKING IN PIGEONS

Variation of Chicken Embryo Development by Temperature Influence. Anna Morgan Miller. Rockdale Magnet School for Science and Technology

STUDENT MANUAL CANINE SEARCH SPECIALIST TRAINING UNIT 8: ADVANCED RUBBLE SEARCH

ORIENTATION TO OCEANIC WAVES BY GREEN TURTLE HATCHLINGS

Back to basics - Accommodating birds in the laboratory setting

Multi-Frequency Study of the B3 VLA Sample. I GHz Data

ESTIMATING NEST SUCCESS: WHEN MAYFIELD WINS DOUGLAS H. JOHNSON AND TERRY L. SHAFFER

206 Adopted: 4 April 1984

Machine Learning.! A completely different way to have an. agent acquire the appropriate abilities to solve a particular goal is via machine learning.

The effects of diet upon pupal development and cocoon formation by the cat flea (Siphonaptera: Pulicidae)

Shooting the poop Featured scientist: Martha Weiss from Georgetown University

Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey

Back to Basics: Mouse Husbandry. Tamara Godbey Clinical Veterinarian University of British Columbia

W. E. CASTLE C. C. LITTLE. Castle, W. E., and C. C. Little On a modified Mendelian ratio among yellow mice. Science, N.S., 32:

GPS in pigeon racing Denmark 2017 Kasper Korndal-Henriksen Ove Fuglsang Jensen

What causes lizards brains to change size?

Writing Simple Procedures Drawing a Pentagon Copying a Procedure Commanding PenUp and PenDown Drawing a Broken Line...

THE PRETRIGEMINAL CAT AS AN INSTRUMENT FOR INVESTIGATION OF THE OCULAR FIXATION REFLEX

MAGNETIC ORIENTATION AND NAVIGATION BEHAVIOR OF LOGGERHEAD SEA TURTLE HATCHLINGS (Caretta caretta) DURING THEIR TRANSOCEANIC MIGRATION

Influence of adult courtship experience on the development of sexual preferences in zebra finch males*

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

Marine Turtles Use Geomagnetic Cues during Open-Sea Homing

International Rescue Dog Organisation. Guideline IRO Team Competition

Avian magnetic compass: Its functional properties and physical basis

Behavioural Processes

THE INDIVIDUALITY OF SOWS IN REGARD TO SIZE OF LITTERS

IMPROVEMENT OF SENSORY ODOUR INTENSITY SCALE USING 1-BUTANOL FOR ENVIRONMENTAL ODOUR EVALUATION

Old and novel methods for estimating Feral Pigeons (Columba livia f. domestica) population size: a reply to Amoruso et al. (2013)

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

Transcription:

The Journal of Experimental Biology 199, 2531 2535 (1996) Printed in Great Britain The Company of Biologists Limited 1996 JEB0542 2531 HOMING BEHAVIOUR OF PIGEONS SUBJECTED TO UNILATERAL ZINC SULPHATE TREATMENT OF THEIR OLFACTORY MUCOSA SILVANO BENVENUTI 1, * AND ANNA GAGLIARDO 2 1 Dipartimento di Scienze del Comportamento Animale e dell Uomo, Università di Pisa, Via Volta 6, I-56126 Pisa, Italy and 2 Centro di Studio per la Faunistica ed Ecologia Tropicali del CNR, Via Romana 17, I-50125 Firenze, Italy Accepted 30 July 1996 Pigeons were made anosmic by unilateral treatment of their olfactory mucosa with a zinc sulphate solution and by plugging the contralateral nostril. In a series of releases at unfamiliar sites, 55 79 km from the home loft, the experimental birds homing behaviour was compared with that of two control groups: unmanipulated control birds, and birds subjected to unilateral zinc sulphate treatment and equipped with an ipsilateral nasal plug. The experimental pigeons exhibited homing behaviour in terms of both homeward initial orientation and homing performance significantly poorer than that of both Summary unmanipulated and treated control pigeons. In addition, the homing behaviour of the treated controls turned out to be only slightly, and not significantly, poorer than that of the unmanipulated birds. The results show that the impaired homing capabilities of the zinc-sulphate-treated birds are due to the lack of navigational information and not to non-specific brain damage caused by the experimental treatment. Key words: pigeon, zinc sulphate, anosmia, olfactory navigation, homing, Columba livia. Introduction During the last two decades, intense investigation of the navigational mechanisms of pigeons has been focused on the olfactory hypothesis developed by Papi s research group in Pisa (references in Papi, 1991, 1995; Wallraff, 1990; Benvenuti et al. 1992b; for debates on controversial aspects of olfactory navigation, see Schmidt-Koenig, 1987; Wiltschko et al. 1987; Waldvogel, 1989; Able, 1996; Wallraff, 1996; Wiltschko, 1996). Some of the many experiments aimed at verifying the role of olfactory information in pigeon navigation were carried out by Schlund and Schmid (Schlund and Schmid, 1991; Schlund, 1992; Schmid and Schlund, 1993), who ran tests with German pigeons made anosmic by bilateral treatment of their olfactory mucosa with zinc sulphate (see Benvenuti et al. 1992a, for references on zinc-sulphate-induced anosmia). These authors admit that their results are suggestive and seem to support the hypothesis of olfactory navigation, but they then conclude that their results may have been produced by a non-specific zinc-sulphate-induced disturbance of the birds behaviour, with possible effects on learning, motivation, circadian rhythms or magnetic perception. Thus, the German authors reject the method they had chosen on the basis of the results they obtained. They propose, against the parsimony principle, new explanations for a phenomenon impairment of navigation following anosmia which had been recorded by several authors with birds made anosmic using different methods (for references, see Papi, 1990). In any case, the implications of the conclusions of Schlund and Schmid are serious for two reasons. First, they suggest that the zinc sulphate method which is frequently used on vertebrates is not a sound one; second, they leave unsettled the problem that they wanted to solve, i.e. whether pigeons in their area use olfactory cues to home. We therefore ran a series of tests aimed at verifying whether the loss of navigational ability in zinc-sulphate-treated birds is actually related to non-specific effects. For this experimental series, we developed a new technique to achieve zinc-sulphateinduced anosmia. We used unilaterally treated control and experimental birds; the former and the latter were then equipped with ipsilateral and contralateral, respectively, nasal plugs. This experimental plan, like that used in previous tests using different methods of olfactory deprivation (Papi et al. 1972; Baldaccini et al. 1975; Benvenuti, 1979), allows one to subject both bird groups (controls and experimentals) to identical experimental manipulations and to a similar exposure to the risk of a non-specific disturbance. The only crucial difference is that the controls, unlike the experimentals, are able to smell through the non-treated side of their olfactory apparatus. *e-mail: benve@discau.unipi.it.

2532 S. BENVENUTI AND A. GAGLIARDO Materials and methods The pigeons and experimental treatment The birds used in our experiments were born in the spring of 1995 in our field station in Arnino (Pisa, Italy) and housed in a loft there. Their pre-test experience was very limited, consisting of spontaneous daily flights and 2 3 training tosses at a maximum distance of about 30 km from the home loft, in different directions. The day before each single test, the birds were subjected to unilateral washing of the olfactory mucosa through one nostril with an aqueous solution of ZnSO 4. 7H 2 O (4 %). In order to avoid possible contamination of the other nostril by gravity or capillarity during the unilateral treatment with zinc sulphate solution, the other nostril was washed with a Ringer s solution. This contemporaneous washing of the nostrils with two different solutions was achieved by using two paired syringes, one of which contained 4 5 ml of zinc sulphate and the other an equal volume of Ringer s solution. The needles of the syringes had been replaced with thin Teflon tubes inserted into the pigeon s nostrils. Gentle pressure on the syringes allowed the solutions to wash the olfactory apparatus from the nostrils to the choanae. In order to avoid the ingestion of the solutions, another operator removed them from the mouth while they were flowing from the choanae by aspiration with a suction pump. The treated birds were then subdivided into two groups, control and experimental birds, which were subjected to ipsilateral (controls) and contralateral (experimentals) plugging of one nostril (ipsilateral and contralateral with respect to the nostril that had received the zinc sulphate solution). The plugs were made with an adequate amount of paste (Xantoprene ) which turns into a solid, rubbery plug after insertion into the nostril. The pigeons were then allotted to treatment groups (controls and experimentals) in such a way as to have an equal number of right-side- and left-side-treated birds in the control and experimental groups. A third group of birds was transported unmanipulated to the release sites. The nasal plugs were removed as soon as the pigeons had homed (no birds lost the plugs, either during the passive displacement or during the homing flight). Release tests and statistical methods The experimental series started when pigeons were 3 4 months old. Each pigeon took part in only one test release. The tests were run on sunny days with no wind or only light winds. The three treatment groups, untreated controls (UC), treated controls (TC) and anosmic experimentals (E), were transported to the unfamiliar release sites in the same well-ventilated van. The birds were released singly, alternating different treatments. Vanishing times and bearings and homing performances were recorded according to standard procedures used in pigeon homing studies. Each set of vanishing bearings was tested for randomness using the Rayleigh and V-tests; the Watson U 2 -test was then used to compare different bearing distributions (Batschelet, 1981). Homing performances of UC, TC and E birds were tested using the Kruskal Wallis analysis of variance by rank (Siegel, 1956) and Dunn s multiple comparison test (Zar, 1984). The homing performances of left-side- and right-sidetreated controls (TC) were compared using the Mann Whitney U-test (Siegel, 1956). Results Initial orientation Table 1 reports the results of the single release tests. In all the tests, both treated (TC) and untreated (UC) controls exhibited better initial homeward orientation than anosmic experimentals (E) in all the four release experiments, as revealed by the higher values of the homeward component (Hc) in the former with respect to the latter. According to the Watson U 2 -test, the difference in initial orientation between the experimentals (E) and each of the two control groups (UC and TC) is significant in all experiments but the fourth, i.e. in six out of eight possible comparisons. Conversely, significant differences between treated (TC) and untreated (UC) controls never occurred, although the homeward orientation of the UC birds was higher in three out of four experiments, as shown by the values of the homeward component (see Table 1). When the vanishing bearings recorded in the complete experimental series were pooled with the home direction set to 0 (Fig. 1), the bearings of both UC and TC birds turned out to be different from random (Rayleigh and V-tests, P<0.001 in both cases) and not significantly different from each other (Watson U 2 -test, P>0.1). The confidence interval of the distributions of both Fig. 1. The vanishing distributions of pigeons subjected to the three treatments (untreated controls, treated controls and experimentals). Each symbol represents the vanishing bearing of a single pigeon; the inner arrows are mean vectors whose length is proportional to the scatter of the related bearing distributions and can be read using the scale in the left-hand diagram. Results from four release experiments are pooled with the home direction (H) set to 0. Other explanations in text. H H H 342 348 244 0 1 Untreated controls Treated controls Experimentals

Unilateral nasal ZnSO 4 treatment in pigeons 2533 Table 1. Results of the experimental series Experiment Tr n (N) (degrees) r Hc U 2 Hp 1 2 3 4 27 June UC 11 (11) 161 0.67** +0.66** NS 8.2.1 152 TC 11 (11) 212 0.74*** +0.37* *** 2.4.5 57km E 11 (11) 324 0.63** 0.62 ** 0.3.8 25 July UC 10 (11) 283 0.94*** +0.92*** NS 5.5.1 293 TC 10 (11) 262 0.78*** +0.67** *** 5.4.2 79km E 11 (11) 181 0.43 0.16 ** 2.1.8 27 July UC 11 (11) 296 0.78*** +0.60** NS 9.2.0 336 TC 11 (11) 296 0.70** +0.53** ** 7.3.1 55km E 11 (11) 231 0.73** 0.19 *** 7.1.3 20 October UC 10 (10) 215 0.85*** +0.75** NS 7.1.2 243 TC 9 (11) 220 0.83*** +0.76** NS 8.1.2 60km E 6 (11) 217 0.80* +0.71** NS 2.2.7 For each of the four experiments, the date of release, the home direction and the distance from home are given. Tr, bird treatment (see text); n, number of vanishing bearings recorded; N, number of birds released; and r, direction and length of the mean vector; Hc, homeward component; U 2, level of statistical significance of the Watson U 2 -test; Hp, homing performance: the three values separated by stops indicate the number of birds that homed on the day of release, the number of birds that homed later and the number of birds lost, respectively. Levels of statistical significance of the Rayleigh (r column), V-test (Hc column) and U 2 -tests are given as asterisks: *P<0.05, **P<0.01, ***P<0.001; NS, not significant. In the U 2 column, the first (top) level of significance refers to the comparison between UC and TC, the second (middle) to that between UC and E, the third (bottom) to that between TC and E. UC, untreated controls; TC, treated controls; E, anosmic experimentals. groups included the home direction (confidence coefficient 0.99 for UC and 0.95 for TC; Hc=+0.73 for TC and +0.57 for UC). Conversely, the bearing distribution of the experimentals, though significantly different from random (Rayleigh test, P<0.01), gives a negative value for the homeward component (Hc= 0.16) and is significantly different from each of the two control groups distributions (Watson U 2 -test, P<0.001, in both comparisons). As reported in the Materials and methods section, treated controls were equipped with an ipsilateral plug inserted into one of the nostrils. As half of the birds were treated with the zinc sulphate solution on the right side and half on the left side, we were able to compare the initial orientation of rightside-treated controls (RT) with that of left-side-treated controls (LT). Fig. 2, in which pooled data are reported, shows that both the RT and LT bearing distributions were different from random (Rayleigh test, P<0.01 and P< 0. 0 5, respectively) and homeward-oriented (the home direction is included in the 95 % confidence interval in both cases; Hc=+0.68 for RT and +0.40 for LT). Despite the apparent difference in the lengths of the mean vectors, the Watson U 2 - test does not reveal any significant difference between the two bearing distributions (P> 0. 0 5 ). Fig. 2. The initial orientation of right-side- and left-side-treated control pigeons. The homing performance of the two treatment groups is given below. For birds that homed on the same day as they were released, symbols are listed according to the homing speed. Later, birds homing later than the first day; Lost, birds lost. Further explanation is given in Fig. 1 and in the text.

2534 S. BENVENUTI AND A. GAGLIARDO 30 25 20 15 10 5 0 Same day Later Lost Fig. 3. Homing performance of the three treatment groups; open, hatched and filled bars denote untreated controls, treated controls and anosmic experimental pigeons, respectively. Further explanation is as in Fig. 2 and in the text. Homing performance The results for the initial orientation are in agreement with those for the homing performance. The Kruskal Wallis analysis of variance revealed a statistically significant difference among the groups in three out of four releases (P<0.02 in experiments 2 and 4; P<0.001 in experiment 1). The comparisons TC versus E and UC versus E revealed a statistically significant difference in two (experiment 2, P<0.01; experiment 4, P<0.025; Dunn s test) and three (experiment 1, P<0.0005; experiments 2 and 4, P<0.025; Dunn s test) releases respectively. A significant difference between treated (TC) and untreated (UC) controls was reported only in the first experiment (P<0.02, Dunn s test). A clear picture emerges from Fig. 3, which shows the pooled homing performance. The analysis of the pooled data showed a significant difference among groups (P<0.0001, Kruskal Wallis analysis of variance). On the whole, the performance of the treated controls is not significantly different from that of the untreated controls (Dunn s test, P>0.25), whereas the homing ability of anosmic experimentals (E) is significantly different from that of both control groups, UC and TC (Dunn s test, P<0.0005 and P<0.0025 respectively). No significant differences were reported between the homing performance of the right-side- and left-side-treated controls (P>0.2, Mann Whitney U-test; see Fig. 2). Discussion Our homing experiments have shown that birds made anosmic by washing the olfactory mucosa on one side and by placing a plug in the contralateral nostril performed very poorly in both initial orientation and homing when compared with both treated and untreated controls. It is worth emphasising that the treated controls were subjected to the same manipulation as the anosmic pigeons, but the plugged nostril was ispilateral to the zinc-sulphate-treated one. Only in the fourth test did the initial orientation of the experimentals not differ from that of the two control groups. This result was attributable to the southwest bias of our pigeons (Ioalè, 1995), which simulated the homeward orientation, and not to an unimpaired navigational ability. In fact, the experimentals homing performance was extremely poor compared with both their own high level of homeward orientation and the homing performance of the treated and untreated controls. The experimental birds homing success was very poor in three out of four tests (first, second and fourth). In these three tests, only four out of 33 pigeons (12.1%) were able to home on the same day as the release; in the third test, the anosmic birds homing success was only slightly and nonsignificantly lower than that of the control birds (seven out of 11 anosmic pigeons, 63.6%, homed on the same day as the release). In order to explain the homing ability of some of the anosmic birds, it is important to take into consideration that the home distance in the tests is not so great as to exclude the possibility that the birds would enter areas flown over during training tosses. As regards the unusually good homing success of the anosmic birds from release site 3, it may not be pure chance that this test site is closer than the others (22km) to the site where most of the birds had been trained and was also in a similar direction with respect to home (336 versus 320 ). In our opinion, the unusually good homing performance of the anosmic birds in the third experiment must be attributed to the above-mentioned cause rather than to a possible inefficiency of the zinc sulphate method (see Benvenuti et al. 1992a). Our results, in agreement with a host of previous findings (see Introduction), confirm the fundamental role of olfactory information in homing when pigeons are released at unfamiliar sites. Considering the lack of effect of zinc sulphate on the navigational abilities of the treated controls, our results cannot be attributed to a non-specific disturbance of the zinc sulphate treatment as suggested by Schlund (1992) and Schmid and Schlund (1993). Their deduction of a non-specific disturbance was based on apparently unrelated facts. Ablation of mouse olfactory bulbs has an effect on circadian rhythms (Possidente et al. 1990) and, hence, Schlund (1992) proposed that washing the olfactory mucosa of pigeons with zinc sulphate would affect their time-compensated sun compass. This connection, however, is not based on any experimental evidence. In addition, Schlund (1992) argued that treatment with zinc sulphate might interfere with the magnetic orientation of pigeons by impairing the ophthalmic nerve which, in the bobolink, was found to respond to magnetic variations (Semm and Beason, 1990). However, it is known that pigeons released under sunny conditions, such as those in Schlund s experiments, rely on their sun compass (Wallcott and Green, 1974; Visalberghi and Alleva, 1979; Füller et al. 1983). It is worth observing that the homing behaviour of treated controls (see pooled results), though significantly better than that of the experimentals, is slightly (and non-significantly) poorer than that reported for untreated controls. This slight difference between untreated and treated controls can be put down to the stress experienced by the latter or to other minor

Unilateral nasal ZnSO 4 treatment in pigeons 2535 non-specific effects of the treatment. We have, however, to consider that the treated controls, unlike the unmanipulated birds, could only rely on half of their olfactory apparatus, and this limitation is a plausible obstacle to optimal perception of olfactory information. A phenomenon of olfactory lateralization was recently observed in chicks (Vallortigara and Andrew, 1994a,b). The authors observed that only when the olfactory input is through the right olfactory pathway do the chicks learn the odour properties of objects in their rearing environment and use olfactory stimuli in choosing the objects on which they had been imprinted (Vallortigara and Andrew, 1994a,b). Conversely, our data show that olfactory stimuli received through the right nostril are as effective as those that reach the brain through the left nostril in eliciting an unimpaired navigational response. This result suggests a lack of lateralization of the pigeons olfactory map. Our results give further support to the olfactory navigation hypothesis and exclude the possibility that the marked impairment of the homing ability of pigeons made anosmic by washing their olfactory mucosa with zinc sulphate can be due simply to a systemic effect. It is therefore reasonable to conclude that our results, in agreement with those found in past years, show that the impaired homing abilities of pigeons, including the German birds used by Schlund (1992), were due to the specific deprivation of olfactory information. We are grateful to F. Papi for his critical reading of an earlier draft of the manuscript. The research was supported by a grant from the Italian Research Council (CNR). References ABLE, K. P. (1996). The debate over olfactory navigation by homing pigeons. J. exp. Biol. 199, 121 124. BALDACCINI, N. E., BENVENUTI, S., FIASCHI, V. AND PAPI, F. (1975). New data on the influence of olfactory deprivation on the homing behavior of pigeons. In Olfaction and Taste V (ed. D. Denton and J. P. Coghland), pp. 351 353. New York, London: Academic Press. BATSCHELET, E. (1981). Circular Statistics in Biology. New York: Academic Press. BENVENUTI, S. (1979). Impaired homing ability in anosmic pigeons. Z. Tierpsychol. 51, 406 414. BENVENUTI, S., IOALÈ, P., GAGLIARDO, A. AND BONADONNA, F. (1992a). Effects of zinc sulphate-induced anosmia on homing behaviour of pigeons. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 103A, 519 526. BENVENUTI, S., IOALÈ, P. AND PAPI, F. (1992b). The olfactory map of homing pigeons. In Chemical Signals in Vertebrates VI (ed. R. L. Doty and D. Müller-Schwarze), pp. 429 434. New York: Plenum Press. FÜLLER, E., KOWALSKI, U. AND WILTSCHKO, R. (1983). Orientation of homing pigeons: compass orientation vs piloting by familiar landmarks, J. comp. Physiol. 153, 55 58. IOALÈ, P. (1995). Preferred compass directions of homing pigeons in Italy. Boll. Zool. 62, 13 21. PAPI, F. (1990). Olfactory navigation in birds. Experientia 46, 352 362. PAPI, F. (1991). Olfactory navigation. In Orientation in Birds (ed. P. Berthold), pp. 52 85. Basel: Birkhauser Verlag. PA P I, F. (1995). Recent experiments on pigeon navigation. In Behavioural Brain Research in Naturalistic and Semi- Naturalistic Settings (ed. E. Alleva, A. Fasolo, H.-P. Lipp, L. Nadel and L. Ricceri), pp. 225 238. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic P u b l i s h e r s. PAPI, F., FIORE, L., F IASCHI, V. AND BENVENUTI, S. (1972). Olfaction and homing in pigeons. Monitore zool. ital. (N.S.) 6, 85 95. POSSIDENTE, B., LUMIA, A. R., MCGINNIS, M. Y., TEICHER, M. H., DELEMOS, E., STERNER, L. AND DEROS, L. (1990). Olfactory bulb control of circadian activity rhythm in mice. Brain Res. 513, 325 328. SCHLUND, W. (1992). Intra-nasal zinc sulphate irrigation in pigeons: effects on olfactory capabilities and homing. J. exp. Biol. 164, 171 187. SCHLUND, W. AND SCHMID, J. (1991). Auswirkungen von ZnSO 4 auf die olfaktorissche Wahrnehmung, die Anfangsorientierung und den Heimkehrerfolg von Brieftauben (Columba livia). Verh. dt. zool. Ges. 84, 360. SC H M I D, J. A N D SC H L U N D, W. (1993). Anosmia in ZnSO 4 - t r e a t e d pigeons: loss of olfactory information during ontogeny and the role of site familiarity in homing experiments. J. exp. Biol. 185, 33 49. SCHMIDT-KOENIG, K. (1987). Bird navigation: has olfactory orientation solved the problem? Q. Rev. Biol. 62, 31 47. SEMM, P. AND BEASON, R. C. (1990). Responses to small magnetic variation by the trigeminal system of the bobolink. Brain Res. Bull. 25, 735 740. SIEGEL, S. (1956). Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co. VALLORTIGARA, G. AND ANDREW, R. J. (1994a). Olfactory lateralization in the chick. Neuropsychologia 32, 417 423. VALLORTIGARA, G. AND ANDREW, R. J. (1994b). Differential involvement of right and left hemisphere in individual recognition in the domestic chick. Behav. Process. 33, 41 58. VISALBERGHI, E. AND ALLEVA, E. (1979). Magnetic influences on pigeon homing. Biol. Bull. mar. biol. Lab., Woods Hole 125, 246 256. WALCOTT, C. AND GREEN, R. P.(1974). Orientation of homing pigeons altered by a change in the direction of an applied magnetic field. Science 184, 180 182. WALDVOGEL, J. A. (1989). Olfactory orientation in birds. Curr. Orn. 6, 269 321. WALLRAFF, H. G. (1990). Navigation by homing pigeons. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 2, 81 115. WALLRAFF, H. G. (1996). Seven theses on pigeon homing deduced from empirical findings. J. exp. Biol. 199, 105 111. WILTSCHKO, R. (1996). The function of olfactory input in pigeon orientation: does it provide navigational information or play another role? J. exp. Biol. 199, 113 119. WILTSCHKO, W., WILTSCHKO, R. AND WALCOTT, C. (1987). Pigeon homing: different effects of olfactory deprivation in different countries. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 21, 333 342. ZAR, J. H.(1984). Biostatistical Analysis. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice- Hall, Inc.