Aggressive behaviour of captive Bandicota bengalensis

Similar documents
P VASANTA KUMARI and JAMIL AHMAD KHAN Department of Zoology, Aligarh Muslim University, Aligarh

Flashing Neon as a Stress Factor Promoting Aggression in the Rat

MSMR Enrichment Symposium, 15 April 2010 MSMR Enrichment Symposium, 15 April 2010

Do male mice prefer or avoid each other s company? Influence of hierarchy, kinship and familiarity

Field evaluation of anticoagulant rodenticides, bromadiolone and difethialone in sugarcane fields of Cauvery delta

Silverback Male Presence and Group Stability in Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla)

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

AnOn. Behav., 1971, 19,

Social Behavior of the Alaska Brown Bear

SOCIOMETRIC INVESTIGATIONS IN GROUPS OF WILD AND DOMESTIC RABBITS WITH ONE BUCK AND TWO OR THREE DOES

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

HUMAN-COYOTE INCIDENT REPORT CHICAGO, IL. April 2014

PAIRING BEHAVIOR OF PIGEONS RELATED TO AGGRESSIVENESS AND TERRITORY

UNIVERSITY OF PITTSBURGH Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee

The Use of Cage Enrichment to Reduce Male Mouse Aggression Neil Ambrose & David B. Morton Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

URINE MARKING OF CAGES AND VISUAL ISOLATION AS POSSIBLE SOURCES OF ERROR IN BEHAVIOURAL STUDIES OF SMALL MAMMALS

Social Housing and Environmental Enrichment Policy

Effects of bedding material and running wheel surface on paw wounds in male and female Syrian hamsters

texp. Biol. (196a), 39,

New York State Mammals. Order Lagomorpha Order Rodentia

Rodent behaviour and handling

Unit 3 Sustainability and interdependence Sub Topic 3.4: Animal welfare

OBSERVATION AND INFERENCE CRITICAL THINKING ACTIVITY

Dealing with the devil

Approving Investigator Managed Use Sites and Housing Areas SOP Number: PURPOSE: 2.0 SCOPE:

Standard Operating Procedure (SOP) APPROVING INVESTIGATOR MANAGED USE SITES AND HOUSING AREAS

Vertebrate Pest Management

Research Article Cause and Possible Treatments of Foot Lesions in Captive Syrian Hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus)

Be Doggone Smart at Work

ILLINO PRODUCTION NOTE. University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Library Large-scale Digitization Project, 2007.

Institutional Animal Care & Use Program - UTEP Title: Animal Enrichment Policy#: 014 Date in Effect: 27 February 2015

Estimating radionuclide transfer to reptiles

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

Formoguanamine-induced blindness and photoperiodic responses in the Japanese quail, Coturnix coturnix japonica

Biology of Citrus Trunk Borer (Anoplophora versteegi Rits.) (Coleoptera : Cerambycidae) under Laboratory Conditions

Parus major L. III. Home range of the basic flocks

Reproductive success and symmetry in zebra finches

King penguin brooding and defending a sub-antarctic skua chick

Impact of Trainings on the Gain in Knowledge of the Field Veterinary Professionals

P.C. RAJE AND V.P. JOSHI

FELINE SOCIAL BEHAVIOUR

Minnesota_mammals_Info_12.doc 11/20/09 -- DRAFT Page 36 of 42

ISAAT species list. Preamble

Scaled Quail (Callipepla squamata)

Care For Us Arc$c Wolf (Canis lupus arctos)

Your Guide To DEFENDING YOUR HOME. Against RATS & MICE

Behavior Modification Why Punishment Should Be Avoided

Winning with warts? A threat posture suggests a function for caruncles in Ross s Geese

Does history-taking help predict rabies diagnosis in dogs?

American Veterinary Society of Animal Behavior: Position Statement on the Use of Dominance Theory in Behavior Modification of Animals

SEXUAL IMPRINTING IN FEMALE ZEBRA FINCHES: CHANGES IN PREFERENCES AS AN EFFECT OF ADULT EXPERIENCE

Biology of rodents and lagomorphs Joanna Godawa Stormark. Taxonomy of rodents. Order: Rodentia (rodents) Class : Mammalia (mammals)

Similipal Tiger Reserve, Baripada, Orissa

Exterior egg quality as affected by enrichment resources layout in furnished laying-hen cages

Digging behavior in 12 taxa of muroid rodents

TABLE 1: NUMBER OF ANIMALS USED IN RELATION TO THEIR PLACE OF ORIGIN

St Margaret College Half Yearly Examinations Name: Class: A: Reading 20 marks HAMSTERS

rodent species in Australia to the fecal odor of various predators. Rattus fuscipes (bush

COMMISSION OF THE EUROPEAN COMMUNITIES REPORT FROM THE COMMISSION TO THE COUNCIL AND THE EUROPEAN PARLIAMENT

STRESS ASSESSMENT OF PIGLETS UTILISING BEHAVIOUR TOOLS UNDER DIFFERENT MANAGEMENTAL PRACTICES*

European Convention for the Protection of Vertebrate Animals used for Experimental and Other Scientific Purposes *

University of Wyoming National Park Service Research Center Annual Report

From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research. Animal behavior then & now

March 16, Guide's space recommendations as a minimum while always recognizing that performance standards also must be met.

SELECTION FOR AN INVARIANT CHARACTER, VIBRISSA NUMBER, IN THE HOUSE MOUSE. IV. PROBIT ANALYSIS

Drexel University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee Mouse Breeding Policy

RABIES CONTROL INTRODUCTION

Management traits. Teagasc, Moorepark, Ireland 2 ICBF

Behaviour of laying curve in Babcock-380 brown commercial layers in Kelantan, Malaysia

Effects of Cage Stocking Density on Feeding Behaviors of Group-Housed Laying Hens

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

Introduction to the Cheetah

Temperature Relationships of Two Oklahoma Lizards

Crepuscular. Well, of course they do that, a friend said. They re crepuscular.

Surveys of the Street and Private Dog Population: Kalhaar Bungalows, Gujarat India

DEMOGRAPHY AND DISPERSAL IH ISLAND AND MAINLAND POPULATIONS OF THE DEER HOUSE, PeromYgcus maniculatus THOMAS PRIESTLAY SULLIVAN

Family Soricidae Masked shrew Southeastern shrew (long-tailed shrews)

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp

Effects of paedomorphosis on signaling behaviors in dyadic encounters of the domestic dog

Diurnal variation in microfilaremia in cats experimentally infected with larvae of

Influence of adult courtship experience on the development of sexual preferences in zebra finch males*

WildlifeCampus Advanced Snakes & Reptiles 1. Vipers and Adders

JOHN PARSONS AND LUIS F. BAPTISTA

Course Offerings: Associate of Applied Science Veterinary Technology. Course Number Name Credits

Academy. Empower Through Training

Canine Aggression SIBLING RIVALRY INDIAN HILLS ANIMAL CLINIC. Indian Hills Animal Clinic

The House Mouse (Mus musculus)

Clinical Practice Guidelines

Back to basics - Accommodating birds in the laboratory setting

Iguana aggression. A relaxed green iguana. Defensive aggression

Feeding behaviour of the Indian musk shrew, Suncus murinus

THE sexual behavior of the common

The organization and control of grooming in cats

PAUL GRIGNON DUMOULIN

Selection of social traits in juvenile Japanese quail affects adults behaviour

The effect of choice-feeding from 7 weeks of age on the production characteristics of laying hens

PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE

Effects of a Pre-Molt Calcium and Low-Energy Molt Program on Laying Hen Behavior During and Post-Molt

RODENTS OF THE GREATER AUCKLAND REGION. by John L. Craig SUMMARY

Cumbria Biodiversity Data Centre Cumbria Mammal Group

Transcription:

Proc. Indian Acad. SCI. (Anim. Sci), Vol. 92, Number 3, May 1983, pp. 185-191 c> Printed in India Aggressive behaviour of captive Bandicota bengalensis 1. Introduction SHAKUNTHALA SRIDHARA and RV KRISHNAMOORTHY Departments of Vertebrate Biology and Zoology, University of Agricultural Sciences, G KV K, Bangalore 560 065, India MS.received 18 October 1982; revised 3 February 1983 Abstract. The aggressive behaviour rhythm of Bandicota bengalensis under different social conditions were studied in the laboratory. The number, intensity and duration of aggressive behaviour peaks varied depending on the number, sex and social status of the interacting conspecific Is. Significantly the peaks of aggression were recorded during the dark period. In heterosexual conflicts both sexes were equally belligerent. However in male interactions with more females, the former emerged dominant always. Similarly one male dominated another in male-male encounters. Confrontation amongst several males in the presence offemale indicated the existence of only alpha and omega males. Keywords. Bandicota bengalensis; aggressive behaviour; male dominance; alpha males; omega males. Circadian rhythms of rodents have been wen documented for behaviour under natural conditions (Marten 1973), for activity movement in cages and mazes (Barnett et ai1975), for feeding and drinking (Possidente and Birnbaum 1979) and for aggressive behaviour(landau 1975; Lerwill1977). Desynchronizationof both metabolic and behavioural rhythms of Peromyscus maniculatus occurred due to social stress (Parr and Andrews 1978). Similar dissociation of activity rhythms due to crowding has been observed by several others (Calhoun 1975; Nygren 1978). In-this article we report alterations in the rhythms of aggressive behaviour of lesser bandicoot rat, Bandicota bengalensis under different social situations. 2. Material and methods Bandicota bengalensis, weighing 0-500 g were housed singly in metal cages (35 x35 x 50 cm) and were fed on rat and mouse feed (Hindustan Lever, India) supple- mented with vitamins and vegetables once a week with ad lib supply of water. Photoperiod was regulated at 12 hr light and 12 hr darkness with the light period beginning at 0600 hours. For recording behaviourthe animals were transferred to an observation chamber, details of which have been describej earlier (Sridhara and Krishnamoorthy 1982). Aggressive behaviour of individual members during encounters between male-female, male-male, female-female, one male-two females and one male-three females were studied. Attempts tc study agonistic behaviour in groups involving' more than one male were unsuccessful as submissive males died within 24 hr after being grouped with dominant males. One member of the pairimale in pair Igroup combinationwas restricted to one portion of the chamber with the other portion housing the 185

1 2M. &crd-r1' I"ll 2 '2 3 LIGHT 9, 01. ; ((I(III!I (I 111116 0600 2400 h<'jr. 52 2 2:f = " i " I cnrcbn, r:erm, il: :I:...dT1IlJ o 4 60 401.!: E S2 3 01 I! I b =, ==rl I I I 1 I 01 0=,,--eO cd] 1 1 Ch-dTI I CD (9 (9 <l 60 2 1 o 5 40-6\ 1(1! d I I I II I I I! I I I I I I I 0600 10 1800 2400 I hrsl 0 1 I I I I I I 1 1I I I 1[b:b--.J I I 1 I I! I I 0600 10 1800 2400 Ihrs) Figures 1-5. Each histogram represents the oscillation of an interacting individual in the cage over a 24 hr period...- 00 0\ <:I) ;J:r s::... ::l i:r <:I)... ::l $::l.. ;;;-... ::l cc-e... '<::

Aggressive behaviour of captive B. bengalensis 187 second member I females. By removing the partition the animals were allowed to interact for 10 min during each hour of the LD cycle. The aggressive behavioural acts and postures namely threat, crawling under lover lunge, chase,attack, fight, boxing and bite (Grant and Mackintosh 1963) were scored for each member of the pair Igroup during these observations. The data were collected under natural daylight during day time and under dim red light during nights. Four replications were. carried out for each type of encounter over a seven day schedule and histograms were computed from the combined means of different individuals. The aggressiveness of either sex was established by student t tests. To recognise ranks of aggressive behaviour, if any, during group encounters, Kruskal-Wallis one-way analysis was carried out (Seigel 1956). 3. Results The results presented in figures I-S illustrate that aggressiveness of bandicoots, motivated through the presence of other conspecifics, is rhythmic, The rhythmicity is either bimodal or unimodal depending on the sex, status and number of interacting conspecifics. However, a 7-day long study indicated that the animals have shown a fairly consistent rhythmicity (9S% rhythms) in their behaviour. Males exhibited a single peak at 0400 hr in male-female encounters (figure 1), at 0100 hr in one male-three females interaction (figure S), but two peaks of equal intensity at 10 and 2100 hr in male-male confrontation (figure 2) and three peaks at 0100, 1300 and 2100 hr in one male-three females group encounter, the last mentioned peak being the most intensive (figure 4). The second male in male-male pair exhibited a single peak of aggressive behaviour at 1700 hr (figure 2). The female in male-female pair and one of the females in the female- female encounter displayed two peaks of aggression, at 0400 and 2100 hr in the former case (figure 1) and at 1300 and20 hr in thelatter(figure 2). Maximum aggressive behaviour was scored foj.: the second female of the female- female (figure 2) and third female of one male-three females encounter at 0100 hr(figure 5). One female of one male-two females conflict exhibited peak aggressive behaviour at 0400 hr while the second female shifted her intensity to 1900 hr (figure 4). Similarly the first and second females of one male-three females interaction spaced out theirpeak aggressive behaviour rhythm at 20 and 1800 hr (figure S). Majority of peaks for aggressive behaviour occurred during the dark phase. Only one of the males of the male-male pairand one of the females of the femalefemale pair displayed afternoon peakswhile the othermale of male-male pairand second female of one male-three females interaction displayed intensive aggressive behaviour at twilight hours. Comparison of aggressive behaviour between the sexes indicated male and female as equally belligerent (P< O.OS, table 1) in male-female clashes while in male-male and female-female clashes, one of them established dominance over the other (P<O.Ol and <0.02, table 1). The male was significantly more aggressive than females in encounters involving more than onefemale (p < 0.01, table 1). No rank order of aggressiveness could be established in the two group clashes (H=3.021 and 4.32,P<0.104). Confrontation ensued between two equally matchedmales immediatelyafter their placement in the observation chamber(figure 6), continued violently(figure 7) till one of them was defeated and assumed submissive posture (figure 8). The latter, though not wounded fatally, succumbed to death within 24 hr. Thus we

188 Shakunthala Sridhara and R V Krishnamoorthy Table 1. Comparison of aggressivebehaviour counts scored by two sexes during different social interactions. Confrontation between Scored by Mean score ± S.E. Cumulative score for the combination Mand F M 1±28 F 233±7.353 MandM M1 741±51* M2 514±35 1255 Fand F F 1 446±48* F2 173±26 619 1Mand2F M 358±29* F 1 66±8 F2 58±6 482 1Mand3F M 487±37* F 1 58±16 F2 73±14 F3 21±23 639 M - male; F = female; * Significantly higher aggressive behaviour counts. Figure 6. Both male rats are equally aggressive. The one crouching in the corner is omega.

Aggressive behaviour of captive B. bengalensis 189 Figure 7. 'Boxing' posture of me conflicting rats. Submissive posture by an omega male in the background, Figure 8. The male on the right is alpha; the one on the left is displaying submissive posture and is omega. Another omega is crouching in the background.

190 Shakunthala Sridhara and R V Krishnamoorthy can assume the winner and loser of this encounter as equivalent to alpha and omega of Rattus noruegicus (Barnett 1975).The third male crouching in a corner (figures 6-8) displayed submissive posture even in the absence of threat! attack and avoided the clashing males, but still died without even being attacked, thus also qualifying for omega status. No betas were identified, which could be either due to the unnatural cage conditions of experimentation or due to the extreme violence of alpha B. bengalensis which does not allow other males to survive in his presence. 4. Discussion Parrack (1966) noticed two peaks of locomotor activity in B. bengalensis, one soon after the dark phase and the other early in the morning. Spillet (1968) failed to establish any rhythm for the agonistic behaviour in grouped B. bengalensis although such activity increased between 00 and 0530 hr. However, Parrack and Thomas (1970) noticed aggressivepeaks for dominant B. bengalensis at 0600 and 0900 hr. The occurrence of nightly peaks of aggression may be a natural corollary of the enhanced general activity of this nocturnal species during dark phase. The present results illustrate both rhythmic and non-rhythmic circadian oscillations of aggressive behaviour of B. bengalensis, which are not stable but are influenced by the number, status and sex of the interacting conspecifics. Similar phase dissociation ofmetabolic and behavioural rhythms of P. maniculatus occurred when intraspecific interactions were enhanced by crowding (Farr and Andrews 1975).Such failure of maintenance of phase relationships was drastic in subordinate animals which could be due to avoidance of more aggressive and dominant cohorts. The great variation seen in the aggressive behaviour rhythms in the present study is a reflection of social interactions! adjustments in the different stress situations of the experiments. The social organization of lesser bandicoots differs from that of Rattus noruegicus in that the females are also aggressive in heterosexual paired encounters. Males can be categorised into alpha and omega as in R. noruegicus. Acknowledgements The authors are indebted to the late Dr KRamakrishnan and Dr R Narayana for their constant encouragement and facilities. Technical assistance of MIs..Govindaraju and Rajanna and financial aid of Ford foundation (Grant No. 660 019), New Delhi, are acknowledged. References Barnett S A 1975 The rat: A study in behaviour, (Chicago: The University of Chicago press) pp. 124-125 Barnett S A, Cowan P E and Prakash I 1975 Circadian rhythms of movements of the house rat, Rattus rattus L.; Indiant. &&p. Biol.13 153-155 Calhoun JB 1975 Social modification of activity rhythms in rodents; Chronobiol. suppl. 1 11 Farr L and Andrews' RV 1978 Rank-associated desynchronizatlon of metabolic and activity rhythms of Peromyscus maniculatus in response to social pressure; Ccmp. Biochem. Physiol.A61 539-542 Grant E C and Mackintosh JH 1963 A comparison of the.social postures of some common laboratory rodents; Behaviour 21 246-259 Landau 1T 1975 Light-dark rhythms in aggressive behaviourofthe male golden hamster; Physiol. Behau. 14767-774. Lerwill C 1977 Diurnal variations in the agonistic behaviour of the golden hamster (Mesocricetus auratus); t. Zool182 159-161

Aggressive behaviour of captive B. bengalensis 191 Marten P 1973 The patterns of Peromyscus activity and their correlation with weather; J. Mammal 54 159-188 Nygren J1978 Inter individual influence on diurnal rhythms of activity in cycling and noncycling populations of the field vole, Microtus agrestis L., Oecologia 35 231-239. Parrack D W 1966 The activity cycle ofthe lesser bandicoot rat Bandicota bengalensis; CU". Sci. 35 544-545 Parrack D Wand Thomas J1970 The behaviour of the lesser bandicoot rat, Bandicota bengalensis (Gray and Hardwickeh J. Bombay Nat. Hist. Soc. 67 67-80 Possidente B and Birnbaum S 1979 Circadian rhythms for food and water consumption in the mouse, Mus musculus; Physiol. Behav. 22 657-660 Siegel S 1956 Non-Parametric Statistics for the behavioural Sciences (New York: McGraw-Hili) pp. Spillet JJ1968 The ecologyof the lesser bandicoot in Calcutta (Bombay: Bombay natural History Society) pp.136-144 Sridhara Sand Krishnarnoorthy RV 1982 Rhythmic oscillations in non-aggressive social behaviour in Bandicota bengalensis; Proc. Indian Acad. Sci. (Anim. Sci.) 91317-322