Ethology of Crayfish. Name. Lab partners: Page 1

Similar documents
ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

UNIT 6 Chapter 14. Coastal Ecosystems: Shrimp Versatile Coastal Critters. Coastal Ecosystems. Learning Outcomes. Chapter 14 Lab/Activity #3

4. List 3 characteristics of all arthropods. a. b. c. 5. Name 3 main arthropod groups.

Review Inverts 4/17/15. What Invertebrates have we learned about so far? Porifera. Cnidaria. Ctenophora. Molluscs

Phylum Arthropoda. Chapter 13 Part 2 of 3

Figure 1. Numerical Distribution of Named Animal Taxa.

A DESCRIPTION OF CALLIANASSA MARTENSI MIERS, 1884 (DECAPODA, THALASSINIDEA) AND ITS OCCURRENCE IN THE NORTHERN ARABIAN SEA

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

Animal Diversity 3. jointed appendages ventral nervous system hemocoel. - marine

UPOGEBIA LINCOLNI SP. NOV. (DECAPODA, THALASSINIDEA, UPOGEBIIDAE) FROM JAVA, INDONESIA

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Territorial signals. Design rules for territorial signals. Why defend a territory? Bird song and territory defense

Iguana aggression. A relaxed green iguana. Defensive aggression

Aggression Social Aggression to Unfamiliar Dogs

Name Class Date. After you read this section, you should be able to answer these questions:

This article originally ran in the March 2013 issue of ASK magazine MANE. Why do lions have manes? One scientist packs up her

NAUSHONIA PAN AMEN SIS, NEW SPECIES (DECAPODA: THALASSINIDEA: LAOMEDIIDAE) FROM THE PACIFIC COAST OF PANAMA, WITH NOTES ON THE GENUS

Class Reptilia Testudines Squamata Crocodilia Sphenodontia

Deer Inquiry: Evolution Why have red deer and elk diverged?

Diplurans. Classification Life History & Ecology Distribution. Major Families Fact File Hot Links

Calming Signals - The Art of Survival

TitleA NEW PORCELLANID CRAB FROM.

Flashing Neon as a Stress Factor Promoting Aggression in the Rat

Protecting Workers in Bear Country

INTRODUCTION & MEASURING ANIMAL BEHAVIOR

How does the rescue and rehabilitation of stranded and injured sea turtles impact species survival? Vocabulary:

Diversity of Animals

A REDESCRIPTION OF THE HOLOTYPE OF CALLIANASSA MUCRONATA STRAHL, 1861 (DECAPODA, THALASSINIDEA)

All living things are classified into groups based on the traits they share. Taxonomy is the study of classification. The largest groups into which

This is interesting. Dogs, like people, use body language to express feelings.

Lab 9: Inventing Life Forms

Amazing arthropods. Kindergarten-Second. Life Science TEKS. Life Science Vocabulary

Effective August 2007 All indicators in Standard / 11

Animal Adaptations. Structure and Function

Sergio, A NEW GENUS OF GHOST SHRIMP FROM THE AMERICAS (CRUSTACEA: DECAPODA: CALLIANASSIDAE)

Proceedings of the World Small Animal Veterinary Association Mexico City, Mexico 2005

Morphologic study of dog flea species by scanning electron microscopy

Management of bold wolves

Livestock - Definition

Biodiversity Trail Birds and Insects

AGGRESSION (CATS) DIAGNOSING AND TREATING

15 years. Name Sex Birth Year Individual History

Directions: Read the passage. Then answer the questions below.

4B: The Pheasant Case: Handout. Case Three Ring-Necked Pheasants. Case materials: Case assignment

Skulls & Evolution. 14,000 ya cro-magnon. 300,000 ya Homo sapiens. 2 Ma Homo habilis A. boisei A. robustus A. africanus

Sea Turtle Strandings. Introduction

THE LARVAL STAGES OF PLEURONCODES PLANIPES STIMPSON (CRUSTACEA, DECAPODA, GALATHEIDAE)

AGGRESSIVE DISPLAY OF THE CORN-CRAKE.

BitttibBxw&tmitivjfjX* -'.'*.w--»<^ ;^M

Vertebrate and Invertebrate Animals

Aggression and social structure

CALLS OF THE WILD Secrets of Animal Speak

Reptiles and amphibian behaviour

Chapter 11: Echinoderms. Spiny-skinned Invertebrates

LABORATORY EXERCISE 6: CLADISTICS I

Echinoderms. Copyright 2011 LessonSnips

Companion Animals. Animal Facilities DOGS. Animal Behavior/Restraint: Companion Animals. General Companion Animal Behavior

ANTHR 1L Biological Anthropology Lab

6-3.4 Physical Responses

Field Herpetology Final Guide

Bear Awareness Training

Feeding Behavior of a Dog, Betta Fish, and Leopard Gecko. Shannon Hutchison

Essential Question: What are the characteristics of invertebrate animals? What are the characteristics of vertebrate animals?

Ardea herodias (Great Blue Heron)

The S Files Success with Maria: Sunshine: Biting Reported by S.G. Friedman, PhD and L. McGuire

Vertebrates. Vertebrate Characteristics. 444 Chapter 14

From The Real Deal on Dogs by David Muriello. How to Choose a Great Dog (The Checklist)

1. Examine the specimens of sponges on the lab table. Which of these are true sponges? Explain your answers.

CLASSIFICATION OF ANIMALS

Phylum Echinodermata. Biology 11

Millipedes Made Easy

Central Marine Fisheries Research Institute, Mandapam Camp

Do You Want to Stop Your Dog Barking and barking.?

Name Class Date. After you read this section, you should be able to answer these questions:

Figure 1 Background Information to the phylum Arthropoda and appears to not have changed for

Species must be adapted to their habitat.

Faculty Mentor, Department of Integrative Biology, Oklahoma State University

Rules to obtain: N.V.B.K. CERTIFICATE Basis Exercises Belgian Ring. Obedience Agility Courage /daring. Directions

CERT Animal Response II

The Development of Behavior

Title. Grade level. Time. Student Target. PART 3 Lesson: Populations. PART 3 Activity: Turtles, Turtle Everywhere! minutes

Animal phyla. Prior Knowledge Questions:

Evaluation of XXXXXXX mixed breed male dog

PORTRAIT OF THE AMERICAN BALD EAGLE

Behavior Modification Why Punishment Should Be Avoided

RESPONSES OF CAMBARID CRAYFISH TO PREDATOR ODOR

FOUR NEW PHILIPPINE SPECIES OF FRESH-WATER SHRIMPS OF THE GENUS CARIDINA

What Can I Learn From a Skull?

There are 35 phyla of animals These phyla can be classified into two groups (vertebrates or invertebrates) based on external and internal physical

Field Trip: Harvard Museum of Natural History (HMNH)

Social Behavior of the Alaska Brown Bear

Claw removal and its impacts on survivorship and physiological stress in Jonah crab (Cancer borealis) in New England waters

Reptile Round Up. An Educator s Guide to the Program

Nature Club. Insect Guide. Make new friends while getting to know your human, plant and animal neighbours!

BREVIORA LEUCOLEPIDOPA SUNDA GEN. NOV., SP. NOV. (DECAPODA: ALBUNEIDAE), A NEW INDO-PACIFIC SAND CRAB. Ian E. Efford 1

Dogs in ATZ (Fan-Based) By John Paul 04/10 Version 1

ADOPTION APPLICATION

People around the world should be striving to preserve a healthy environment for both humans and

ANTI-DOG ENFORCEMENT - What Every Dog Owner Needs to Know

Behavioral Study of Sand Fiddler Crabs: Uca pugilator. Sara Snider, Ruth Adkinson, Sarah Bailey, and Sarah Hunt

Transcription:

wrong 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 right 101 100 99 98 97 96 95 94 93 92 91 90 89 88 87 86 85 84 83 82 81 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 score 100 99 98 97 96 95 94.1 93.1 92.1 91.1 90.1 89.1 88.1 87.1 86.1 85.1 84.2 83.2 82.2 81.2 80.2 79.2 78.2 77.2 76.2 75.2 74.3 73.3 72.3 71.3 70.3 Ethology of Crayfish Name Lab partners: Responding to environmental stimuli is one of the fundamental principles that define living organisms. For animals, the ability to detect and respond to stimuli is critical because an incorrect response can mean death for an individual. The type of environmental stimuli that animals respond to include abiotic or non-living stimuli; these include the rising or setting of the sun, changing seasons, and in marine organisms, the movement of the tides. Animals also respond to biotic or living environmental stimuli. Biotic stimuli can include, but are not limited to, the presence of predators, food, mates or other family members. For this laboratory exercise we will observe, quantify, and analyze the behaviors exhibited between crayfish and their environment and between two crayfish competitors. It is always in the best interest for two animals, upon meeting, to "decide" which one of the two is superior (i.e. dominant, stronger, territory holder) without a significant physical altercation. The behaviors that animals use to decide these meetings are classified as agonistic behaviors, which can include aggressive or submissive behaviors. By deciding which of the two individuals is superior without a physical altercation, the fitness of both individuals is increased because their probability of survival is increased; even the winner in a fight may be injured or mortally wounded in the combat. Information that is sent from one individual to another may be classified as honest or dishonest. An honest signal is one that cannot be faked or copied in any way. For instance male red deer, Cervus elaphus, compete for mates by roaring; the male making the deepest and longest roar is the winner and gains access to the females in the herd. In this example small males cannot fake a loud, deep call because a larger healthy male (with greater lung capacity) will always win roaring contests. This is why the signal is called honest. There are many types of honest signals in the animal kingdom including special coloration or plumage in birds and manes or distinct fur in mammals. In contrast to an honest signal, a dishonest signal is falsehood or bluff. This strategy must be employed sparingly during specific times of their lives because it is based on a lie. An example of a dishonest signal may be seen in some crustaceans, such as the American lobster Homarus americanus. The lobster, like all crustaceans, must molt to grow. The postmolt period (shortly after an individual has molted and lost its shell) in the life of a lobster is one in which the newly molted individual is particularly vulnerable, even if they are large. Just after a lobster has molted, the postmolt animal has a very soft shell and poorly developed musculature to support its claws and the rest of the body. As a result, any physical altercation that a postmolt lobster enters into will almost certainly result in a combat loss and even death. Lobsters in this state (postmolt status) adopt behaviors that account for their vulnerability. One way to do this is to present their opponent with a dishonest signal or a bluff. When two lobsters meet for the first time and one lobster is postmolt (soft shell) and the other is a hard shell animal (also known as intermolt or premolt status) then competitive bouts are different than in interactions between two hard shell lobsters. The postmolt individual will often forego a characteristic series of agonistic behaviors and will rapidly escalate an encounter. This strategy presents the other combatant (the hard shell combatant) with a choice to escalate the fight with a potentially larger individual or retreat. If the hard shell combatant retreats, then the bluff was a success. If the hard shell combatant continues the fight, then the postmolt (soft) lobster retreats quickly. Experimental evidence has demonstrated that this is indeed what postmolt individuals choose to do: rapidly escalate and, if faced with further combat, retreat as quickly as possible. It is always better to run or, in this case, swim away from a fight so that you survive to fight another day. For this laboratory exercise we will study the freshwater crayfish Procambarus clarkii, a close relative of the American lobster. Both are members of the decapod crustaceans, or individuals with 10 walking legs (5 pairs, one set of 5 on each side). They both have a hard exterior shell known as an exoskeleton that covers their cephalothorax, walking legs and abdominal segments. The cephalothorax is the anterior-most portion of the body and includes the eyes, mouth, two long mechanoreceptor antennae and two pairs of olfactory antennules at the front of the animal. Individual body size is defined as the distance between one eye socket and the end of the cephalothorax, and is called the carapace length (CL) and is expressed in millimeters. This document Ross E. Koning 1994. Permission granted for non-commercial instruction. Koning, Ross E. 1994. Ethology of Crayfish. Plant Physiology Information Website. http://plantphys.info/organismal/labhtml/crayfish.html. (8-23-2013). Page 1

Page 2 Ventral to the cephalothorax are 5 pairs of legs; the first pair (also known as the claw or merus) is larger than the other four pairs. The final four pairs of legs are more similar to one another with the last pair of legs being attached just in front of the abdomen; these are used for walking. The abdomen or tail area is a broad, segmented section at the posterior of the animal and terminates (ends) with a single pair of uropods on either side of a central telson (Fig. 1). carapace length Figure 1. External anatomy and morphology of a male crayfish Procambarus clarkii. Figures 1A and B illustrate the dorsal and ventral views respectively. Carapace length (CL) is defined as the distance between the eye socket and the end of the carapace (Figure A). Please note that in figure 1B, the paired first swimmerets of the male are stout and are used as a modified penis. In the female the first pair of swimmerets is soft and identical to swimmerets 2 to 5. You should use the presence/absence of this structure to determine the sex of the study individuals. http://crayfish.byu.edu/crayfish_biology.htm Laboratory Procedure Observing the Animal: The first thing to do is to get to know one of the crayfish from the community tank and observe the important anatomy of the animal. First, focus on the anterior region where much of the sensory information is gathered by the animal. You should see eyes, antennae, antennules and mouth parts. The eyes are paired and found on either side of a central rostrum, or horn. The eyes are stalked and each eye has several hundred individual eyes (ommatidia). Having so many individual eyes allows the individual to survey its environment and avoid being attacked from behind.

Page 3 How many distinct kinds of antennae do you see on the animal? What is the major difference between the different kinds of antennae? Which of the kinds of antennae are used for mechnaoreception? antennae antennules Which of the kinds of antennae are used for olfaction? antennae antennules Describe the difference you observe among the maxillipeds. One pair is The others are comparatively How many pairs of legs does the crayfish have? Are the pairs of legs identical or differently shaped or constituted? identical different What is obviously different about the first pair of legs? What do the remaining more-anterior walking legs have, not found on the more-posterior legs? The remaining more-anterior legs have: What is the sex of your study organism? male female What is the carapace length of your study organism? ( ) length (units) Crayfish Response to Potential Predators: Crayfish are epibenthic inhabitants of the freshwater community and can live in many different environments including rivers, lakes and streams. Because of the diversity of their habitats and their general size (just more than a mouthful for many predators), it is critical for a crayfish to detect and respond to threats from a variety of different sources. The three most effective nonhuman predators on crayfish are fish, birds and mammals (e.g. raccoons). With the crayfish resting on the table and you holding still, observe the animal's behavior for a few moments. Using your arms and hands, reach toward the crayfish. How does it respond? What is it doing with its walking legs? What is it doing with the chelipeds? What is it doing with its antennae? What is the position of the tail? If it attempted locomotion, which direction did it move? The most common response to a potential predator is called the meral spread. The behavior increases the chances for survival by making the crayfish look larger and placing its nasty defenses in striking position of a tender nose! In observing our crayfish interact with each other today, it is important to minimize any responses to our presence. Holding still, keeping quiet, standing at a distance, avoiding sight-lines are all good behaviors for ethologists! /18

Page 4 In its simplest form, an ethogram is a quantitative description of an animal's behavior. The scientist hopes that the behavior being observed is "natural" but it should be noted that the simple act of observing an animal may influence the behavior of the animal. When you are conducting your observations, make sure to speak softly to your colleagues, to make your observations from a distance, and to move around as little as possible. The objective of this section of the laboratory exercise is to recognize and differentiate crayfish behaviors as they interact. Finally we want you to be able to quantify the frequency, duration and potentially the order in which behaviors take place. Agonistic Interactions: Crayfish respond to potential competitors with characteristic displays particularly if those threats come from conspecifics (another crayfish). Table 1. Agonistic behaviors often observed in interacting crayfish, Procambarus clarkii. Aggressive Submissive Upright posture-cephalothorax being Declined posture-cephalothorax being returned to lifted off substrate resting on substrate Abdomen Unfolding Abdomen Folding-folding telson under Retreat-backs away from opponent Approach-moves toward opponent Tail Flip-jets away from opponent Strike-hits opponent with body part Cheliped Release-lets go of opponent Grasp-grabs or holds opponent Cheliped Flexed-folds cheliped to/under rostrum Push-uses legs to move opponent Agonistic Behavior Observations: You will now observe interactions between two crayfish first-hand. In a tank called the arena, you will match its resident crayfish with different intruder crayfish and document the behavioral interactions between the individuals. To do a test such as this, you should consider factors including the size, health, molt status and sex of the combatants. Obviously if we are hoping to observe agonistic rather than mating behaviors, we would use crayfish of the same gender in each bout, for example. So our independent variable might be size or "previous experience." The dependent variable would be the aggressive and submissive behaviors shown in Table 1. On your bench you have a large tank marked "arena" with just one resident crayfish; this animal has been living alone in this tank for several days and will not be removed from its arena except once briefly for you to determine its carapace length (CL) and sex. This animal will participate in all three bouts as the resident of the arena. On your bench you also have a small tank containing an intruder crayfish that has solitary ownership as a resident of its own tank. This animal will participate as an intruder in two bouts (the first and third ones) with the resident in the arena. You will select a member of the large "community" tank to serve as the intruder for the second bout with the resident in the arena. This animal has been sharing space with many other crayfish, and thus has lots of experience in agonistic behavior. It may be smaller than the resident as well. Before each bout, record the sex, and measure the carapace length of the intruder and resident. You only need to measure and sex each animal carefully just once. Thereafter you can trust your ability to distinguish the animals.

Page 5 The laboratory group of four people should have two watches, two datasheets, and two pencils. One pair of students will observe the resident crayfish and the other pair will watch the intruder for each bout. If there is a white PVC tube initially in the arena, remove it for the rest of the class period; you can return it at the end of bout #3. Place the intruder crayfish in the middle of the arena containing the resident and move your hand and body away quickly. It is important that the observers do not get so close to the arena that the crayfish begin to react to the human rather than to each other! The corrugated box and cover can help shield the arena to keep the influence of the researchers to a minimum. For each bout, lasting 10 minutes, one pair of experimenters will observe and record the duration (in seconds) or the count (in numbers) of the behaviors shown by the resident crayfish, while the other pair of experimenters will observe and record the duration (in seconds) or the count (in numbers) of the behaviors shown by the intruder crayfish. Obviously the person observing will be dictating what s/he observes; the person recording will need to watch the clock and write in the durations or counts of each behavior that have been observed during each minute of the bout. It is possible that a crayfish may not demonstrate any of the behaviors (in which case, the row may contain all dash marks). At the end of the 10 1-minute intervals, carefully remove the intruder crayfish and place it back into its original container (either the "solitary" or the "community" tank). We want to let this crayfish go back to being a "solitary" or a "community" crayfish. Between each bout, it is important to let the resident crayfish recover for 10 minutes. During this recovery time for the resident crayfish, try not to disturb the animal at all. It is best to back away from the arena entirely. As a group you should share your data and answer the follow-up questions during the resting periods between bouts. Remember, that you are not to have any role in the behaviors between the crayfish; please do not "play" with the crayfish (even those not involved) during the bouts, nor during the resting intervals. Long term responses to your harassment, can strongly influence the outcomes of later bouts! Please make sure any lab partners who have not read these instructions are fully compliant with this important directive! Effective lab partners do not allow unprepared partners to wreck the team's projects!

Page 6 Agonistic Behavior Bout #1 In this chart record the seconds (s) of time spent or the number (#) of events for each crayfish performing each behavior during each one-minute interval. A dash ( ) is a reasonable entry if a behavior is not performed during the interval. Sex M F Arena Resident CL mm Sex M F Solitary Intruder CL mm 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 6 6 7 7 8 8 9 9 10 10 Which crayfish won this bout? resident draw intruder Which of the resident's behaviors was most influential in your decision? Which of the intruder's behaviors was most-influential in your decision? /29

Page 7 Agonistic Behavior Bout #2 In this chart record the seconds (s) of time spent or the number (#) of events for each crayfish performing each behavior during each one-minute interval. A dash ( ) is a reasonable entry if a behavior is not performed during the interval. Arena Resident Sex M F Community Intruder CL mm 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 6 6 7 7 8 8 9 9 10 10 Which crayfish won this bout? resident draw intruder Which of the resident's behaviors was most influential in your decision? Which of the intruder's behaviors was most-influential in your decision? How was this bout different from the first? /28

Page 8 Agonistic Behavior Bout #3 "The Rematch" In this chart record the seconds (s) of time spent or the number (#) of events for each crayfish performing each behavior during each one-minute interval. A dash ( ) is a reasonable entry if a behavior is not performed during the interval. Arena Resident Solitary Intruder 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 6 6 7 7 8 8 9 9 10 10 Which crayfish won this bout? resident draw intruder Which of the resident's behaviors was most influential in your decision? Which of the intruder's behaviors was most-influential in your decision? How was this rematch different from their first? /26