Wilson Bull., 98(2), 1986, pp

Similar documents
BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

THE BEGGING BEHAVIOR OF NESTLING EASTERN SCREECH-OWLS

DO BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS?

Growth and Development. Embryonic development 2/22/2018. Timing of hatching. Hatching. Young birds and their parents

THE YOUNG COWBIRD: AVERAGE OR OPTIMAL NESTLING?

Differences in begging behaviour between barn swallow, Hirundo rustica, nestlings

ANALYSIS OF GROWTH OF THE RED-TAILED HAWK 1

Management, Univ. California at Berkeley, Berkeley, California Accepted 15 Oct

PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE

BREEDING ROBINS AND NEST PREDATORS: EFFECT OF PREDATOR TYPE AND DEFENSE STRATEGY ON INITIAL VOCALIZATION PATTERNS

Nestling growth in the Great Tit Parus major and the Willow Tit P. montanus

A future cost of misdirected parental care for brood parasitic young?

DO DIFFERENT CLUTCH SIZES OF THE TREE SWALLOW (Tachycineta bicolor)

EGG SIZE AND LAYING SEQUENCE

SEASONAL PATTERNS OF NESTING IN THE RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD MORTALITY

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns

The effect of testosterone injections on aggression and begging behaviour of black headed gull chicks (Larus ridibundus)

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

SHORT COMMUNICATIONS 757

Toledo, Ohio. The population was located within the city limits

TEMPORAL PATTERNS IN PRE-FLEDGLING SURVIVAL AND BROOD REDUCTION IN AN OSPREY COLONY

ASPECTS OF THE BREEDING BIOLOGY AND PRODUCTIVITY OF BACHMAN S SPARROW IN CENTRAL ARKANSAS

PREDATION ON RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD EGGS AND NESTLINGS

RECOGNITION OF NEST, EGGS, NEST SITE, AND YOUNG IN FEMALE RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS

REGIONAL VARIATION IN COWBIRD PARASITISM OF WOOD THRUSHES

The evolution of conspicuous begging has been a topic of

Factors Influencing Local Recruitment in Tree Swallows, Tachycineta bicolor

Effects of Parasitism by Brown-headed Cowbirds May Persist into Post-fledging

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

Growth and Development of the Black-eared Kite Milvus migrans lineatus

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp

REPRODUCTIVE SUCCESS OF THE NORTHERN CARDINAL, A LARGE HOST OF BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS

Wilson Bull., 94(2), 1982, pp

Offspring sex ratio in red-winged blackbirds is dependent on

FOREIGN OBJECTS IN BIRD NESTS

THE HOUSE SPARROW (PASSER DOMESTICUS)

Male parental care and monogamy in snow buntings

769 q 2005 The Royal Society

HOST-PARASITE INTERACTIONS OF BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS AND DARK-EYED JUNCOS IN VIRGINIA

Does begging affect growth in nestling tree swallows, Tachycineta bicolor?

Incubation feeding in snow buntings: female manipulation or indirect male parental care?

Below, we present the methods used to address these objectives, our preliminary results and next steps in this multi-year project.

GROWTH AND SEXUAL DIMORPHISM BOAT-TAILED GRACKLE

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

Variation in egg mass in the Pied Flycatcher, Ficedula hypoleuca: An experimental test of the brood survival and brood reduction hypotheses

INHERITANCE OF BODY WEIGHT IN DOMESTIC FOWL. Single Comb White Leghorn breeds of fowl and in their hybrids.

Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) are breeding earlier at Creamer s Field Migratory Waterfowl Refuge, Fairbanks, AK

The behaviour of a pair of House Sparrows while rearing young

RESPONSES OF BELL S VIREOS TO BROOD PARASITISM BY THE BROWN-HEADED COWBIRD IN KANSAS

CHICK GROWTH, SIBLING RIVALRY, AND CHICK

COWBIRD PARASITISM AND EVOLUTION OF ANTI-PARASITE STRATEGIES IN THE YELLOW WARBLER

REPORTS BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS SKEW HOST OFFSPRING SEX RATIOS. Department of Biology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6A 5B7, Canada 2

EVALUATION OF A METHOD FOR ESTIMATING THE LAYING RATE OF BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS

Is asynchronous hatching adaptive in herring gulls (Larus argentatus)?

Investigating the role of call similarity in a tree swallow (Tachycineta bicolor) parent-offspring recognitionmechanism

Barn Swallow Nest Monitoring Methods

Coots Use Hatch Order to Learn to Recognize and Reject Conspecific Brood Parasitic Chicks

The Hatching, Growth, and Fledging of Nestling Purple Martins

PARENT-OFFSPRING RECOGNITION IN BANK SWALLOWS (RIPARIA RIPARIA ): I. NATURAL HISTORY

Factors Influencing Egg Production

University of Groningen

Yellow-throated and Solitary Vireos in Ontario: 4. Egg Laying, Incubation and Cowbird Parasitism

Ames, IA Ames, IA (515)

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories

PRODUCTION AND SURVIVAL OF THE VERDIN

SLEEPING BEHAVIOR OF PURPLE MARTINS

Siblicide at Northern Goshawk Nests: Does Food Play a Role?

You may use the information and images contained in this document for non-commercial, personal, or educational purposes only, provided that you (1)

BLACK OYSTERCATCHER NEST MONITORING PROTOCOL

Perceived risk of ectoparasitism reduces primary reproductive investment in tree swallows Tachycineta bicolor

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Laying May May 2 to 26. Incubation Early May to mid June Early May to mid June 30 to 34

The relationship between defecation and feeding in nestling birds: observational and experimental evidence

Purple Martin. Adult male Purple Martin

Rock Wren Nesting in an Artificial Rock Wall in Folsom, Sacramento County, California

ESTIMATING NEST SUCCESS: WHEN MAYFIELD WINS DOUGLAS H. JOHNSON AND TERRY L. SHAFFER

Nest switching and alloparental care in colonial white storks

Adjustment Factors in NSIP 1

This article is downloaded from.

GULLS (LARUS ARGENTATUS)

VARIATION IN INCUBATION PERIOD WITHIN A POPULATION OF THE EUROPEAN STARLING ROBERT E. RICKLEFS AND CYNTHIA

STATUS SIGNALING IN DARK-EYED JUNCOS

Multiple broods from a hole in the wall: breeding Red-and-yellow Barbets Trachyphonus erythrocephalus in southeast Sudan

A COMPARATIVE STUDY OF COWBIRD PARASITISM IN YELLOW-HEADED BLACKBIRDS AND RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS CATHERINE P. ORTEGA AND ALEXANDER CRUZ

Relationship Between Eye Color and Success in Anatomy. Sam Holladay IB Math Studies Mr. Saputo 4/3/15

Reproductive success and symmetry in zebra finches

First contact: A role for adult-offspring social association in the species recognition system of brood parasites

Avian species as indicators of ecosystem health in the Tittabawassee/Saginaw river watershed

AS91603 Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants & animals to their external environment

BLUEBIRD NEST BOX REPORT

P. Chudasama and *P. Dodia Department of Zoology, Sir P.P. Institute of Science, MK Bhavnagar University *Author for Correspondence

Nest size in monogamous passerines has recently been hypothesized

doi: /osj.9.161

T HE recent and interesting paper by Alexander F. Skutch (1962) stimulated

HABITAT AS A PREDICTOR OF HATCH SYNCHRONY IN THE BROWN- HEADED COWBIRD

PROGRESS REPORT for COOPERATIVE BOBCAT RESEARCH PROJECT. Period Covered: 1 April 30 June Prepared by

AnOn. Behav., 1971, 19,

RELATIONSHIPS AMONG WEIGHTS AND CALVING PERFORMANCE OF HEIFERS IN A HERD OF UNSELECTED CATTLE

OF THE HOUSE FINCH AS A HOST OF THE BROWN-HEADED COWBIRD

Transcription:

GENERAL NOTES Wilson Bull., 98(2), 1986, pp. 286-291 Distribution of food within broods of Barn Swallows.-The delivery of food by parent birds and its distribution among nestlings of a brood are important determinants of nestling survival. Despite the role of parental distribution of food among nestlings in the process of brood reduction and subsequent evaluations offitness, direct observations ofparent-offspring interactions in the nest are rare (Lohrl 1968, Best 1977, Ryden and Bengtsson 1980, Reed 1981). Field observations have shown that older and larger nestlings are more likely to survive than are their smaller siblings (Howe 1976, O Connor 1978a, Richter 1982). This could result from parents selectively feeding larger nestlings (Ricklefs 1965, Dawkins 1976) or from the superior competitive abilities of larger nestlings (Lahrl 1968, Richter 1984). Clearly, both processes can operate simultaneously. If parent birds are capable of preferentially feeding some nestlings, then they must be able to distinguish among the nestlings of their brood. Such perceptual capabilities are in stark contrast to what many species seem to possess; for example, consider the inability of many passerines to distinguish Brown-headed Cowbird (Molothrus ater) and other social parasitic nestlings from their own offspring (Rothstein 1982). Beecher and his co-workers (Beecher et al. 198 1 a, b, Stoddard and Beecher 1983) have shown that in colonial Cliff (Hirundo pyrrhonota) and Bank (Riparia ripnria) swallows, parental recognition of individual young occurs when the young are mobile enough to intermingle. Individually distinctive signature calls develop in chicks at about 15 days posthatch. Their observations show that the subtle differences among conspecific nestlings in the nest are unnoticed by parents. Internestling competition is aggressive in raptors (O Connor 1978b, Stinson 1979) but relatively passive in passerines (Skutch 1976). Within a passerine brood, larger nestlings can gape higher and command more favorable positions within the nest than their smaller siblings (LBhrl 1968, Ryden and Bengtsson 1980). A size differential among nestlings often results in the early death of the smallest nestlings (Richter 1984); however, the size of a nestling is not always correlated with the proportion of feedings it receives. For instance, Best (1977) in Field Sparrows (Spizellapusilla) and Reed (198 1) in Song Sparrows (Melospizu melodia) determined that, the sequence of feedings was essentially random. Ryden and Bengtsson (1980) observed that increased begging rates and movement toward favored nest positions enhanced the feeding rate of small nestlings. Methods. -The observations we report on here are of the interactions between parental Barn Swallows (Hirundo rustica) and nestlings at six nests, three each in 1980 and 198 1. The study was conducted in Jefferson County, Kansas, on the Nelson Environmental Studies Area of the University of Kansas, Lawrence. In previous years, Barn Swallows built their nests on the underside of a loft in a barn on the study area. During the fall of 1979, we cut a 7.6-cm diameter hole through the planks above 13 nests that had been active in the past summer, and then filled the hole with a wooden plug. Nest contents were checked regularly until the appearance of the first egg, after which checks were made daily. On the first hatch day, the wooden plug was replaced with a glass plate. Nestlings were marked for individual recognition by painting nail polish on their crowns; marks were replaced periodically as needed. The nesting pair was given a day to adjust to the plate before observations began on day one posthatch. We observed the birds behavior through the glass plate over each nest. Upon our arrival at a nest, parent swallows were momentarily agitated, but quickly habituated to our presence. We did not record data until the birds were calm. We recorded the time of each feeding, which nestlings gaped as the parents arrived at the nest, which nestling was fed, and various nestling behavioral 286

GENERAL NOTES 287 TABLE 1 PROPORTIONOF FEEDINGS GIVEN TO EACH NESTLING AS A FUNCTION OFNESTLING RANK NH Nestling rank rn- Year ber I 2 3 4 5 6 CI' 1980 1 0.35 (200) 2 0.29 (91) 3 0.19 (41) 1981 4 0.22 (16) 5 0.32 (33) 6 0.28 (51) 0.37 (212) 0.35 (111) 0.11 (24) 0.30 (22) 0.33 (34) 0.32 (53) 0.27 (154) 0.36 (113) 0.16 (34) 0.27 (20) 0.35 (36) 0.20 (37) * 95% confidence interval around expected random distnbution. b Number of feedings. r House Sparrow nestling. (0.29-0.37) (0.284.38) 0.15 0.17 0.21 (0.12-0.22) (31) (36) (45) 0.22 (0.15-0.35) (16) (0.24-0.42) 0.22 _ (0.19-0.31) (40) patterns such as preening and defecation. It was impossible to keep track of nestling gaping at Nest 3 in 1980 because it contained six nestlings. To test parental recognition of nestlings, a House Sparrow (Passer domesticus) nestling was added to Nest 6 in 1981 on day two posthatch. Its weight (4.5 g) was equal to that of the lightest Barn Swallow nestling. Two other House Sparrow nestlings were added to two other Barn Swallow nests (not monitored as part of the main study) in 1981 on day nine posthatch and the reaction of the adult and nestling swallows to the intruders noted. Quantitative data reported here are based on 1450 feedings of the nestlings at the six nests. Nestlings were weighed to the nearest 0.5 g with a 25-g Pesola scale every second or third day beginning within one day of hatch (except Nest 3 where nestlings were Iirst weighed on day four). Weighings were discontinued after day 12-l 5, as nestlings approached fledging, to avoid causing premature departure from the nest. Weights on other days were estimated by linear extrapolation between known weights; we assumed a constant age-specific growth rate between consecutive weighing days. This approach has been used by Crossner (1977) to estimate parameters of logistic growth curves. For our purposes, we needed only to be able to rank the nestlings by weight. Nest 1 had the greatest weight difference between highest and lowest ranked nestlings (5 g on day three, increasing to a maximum of 10 g on day seven, but reduced to 1 g on day 17). By comparison, the other five nests showed maximum weight differentials of 3.0, 7.5, 2.5, 4.5, and 5.0 g, respectively. Results.-Our main interest was in determining whether the rank of a nestling (where nestlings were ranked by weight) influenced the nestling s probability of receiving food from parents. To that end we addressed the following questions: (1) Did all nestling ranks receive an equal proportion of the feedings? Because a nestling s rank sometimes changed during the nestling period, the data for a particular rank included

288 THE WILSON BULLETIN l Vol. 98, No. 2, June 1986 TABLE 2 QIRRELATIONS BETWEEN BARN SWALLOW BREAD AGE AND PROPORTION OF DAILY FEEDINGS GIVEN TO NESTLINGS OF HIGHEST AND LOWEST RANK Nestling rank YeaI Nest number Hiahest Lowest 1980 1-0.85 (12)b 0.70 (12) 2 0.07 (10) -0.19 (10) 3 0.07 (8) -0.33 (8) 1981 4-0.76 (7) 0.14 (7) 5-0.30 (11) 0.63 (11) 6 0.16 (9) -0.09 (9) * Spearman rank correlation coefficient is significantly different from 0.0 at P < 0.01. b Number of days observed. c Spearman rank correlation coefficient is signdicantly different from 0.0 at P < 0.05. observations from several nestlings. We compared the proportion of total feedings given to each nestling rank against the null hypothesis of an equal distribution to each rank using a t-test for proportions (Walpole and Myers 1978). Only for Nest 1 did any of the proportions exceed the approximate 95% confidence interval around the expected random distribution of feedings to all nestling ranks (Table 1). Hence, based solely on total feedings, high rank does not seem to confer an advantage to nestlings. (2) Does the potential value of high rank change with nestling age? Due to asynchronous hatching, the within-brood coefficient of variation of nestling weight can be high, but it drops for surviving broods as weights of nestlings converge near fledging. Hence, the relative weight difference between the highest and lowest ranked nestlings in a brood usually decreases as nestlings age. To determine whether the significance of high or low rank to feeding probabilities changed with nestling age, we looked at the correlation between nestling age and the proportion of total daily feedings received for the nestlings of highest and lowest rank in each brood. Again at only one nest, Nest 1, does the importance of rank clearly change with age (Table 2). Here, the highest ranked nestling received a high proportion of the feedings at an early age with the lowest ranked receiving progressively more as the brood aged. Similar trends occurred in nests 4 and 5 in 1981. (3) A nestling has to gape to be fed, but a gape does not guarantee a feeding. Does rank confer an advantage such that a nestling of high rank will be fed a greater proportion of times following a gape than one of low rank? Using ANOVA, we compared the average conditional probability of a nestling being fed given that it gaped (total number of feedings/ total number of gapes) among groups of nestlings ranked by weight. To keep the ANOVA balanced, and in order to include all nests where gapes were recorded, only nestlings ranked one through three were compared. To avoid bias due to absolute feeding rate or food abundance differences, proportions were expressed as deviations from the mean for each brood. Even the highest ranking nestling was passed over from 40 to 52% of the times it gaped for food (Table 3). The ANOVA summary shows that the mean probability of a nestling being fed following a gape is different between ranks (Table 3). The standardized group means are similar for ranks one and two, whereas the mean is lower for nestlings of Rank 3 (X, = 0.028, R, = 0.033,.?, = -0.06). Hence, there is some indication that, compared to high-ranked individuals, lower-ranked nestlings were less likely to be fed when they gaped.

GENERAL NOTES 289 TABLE 3 PROBABILITY OF A NESTLING BEING FED FOLLOWING A GAPE Nestling ranlr Year Nest number I 2 3 4 1980 1 0.57 0.45 0.32 2 0.56 0.56 0.32 1981 4 0.48 0.59 0.55 0.50 5 0.60 0.68 0.60 6 0.49 0.52 0.37 0.43b l Difference among ranks 1 to 3 is significant by ANOVA at P < 0.0 I; Mean Square Ranks/Mm Square Error = 0.0379/ 0.00117 = 32.3 = Fz,,>. b House Sparrow nestling. (4) Can defecation influence feeding probabilities? Even at one day after hatching, each nestling oriented its cloaca toward the outside of the nest prior to defecating. Initially, this facilitated parental removal of the fecal sacs as defecation occurred in the presence of an adult. Later, the nestling defecated over the front of the nest. This turning was very rapid and took place even on top of the other nestlings. Surprisingly, an equally rapid return to a forward-facing position was not observed until nestling day 12. Once oriented to the rear of the nest, the nestlings gradually returned to a forward-facing position, but up to 30 min elapsed before a return was achieved. Whatever the mechanism, the consequence was that, regardless of size, no nestling could maintain an advantageous position in the nest as defecation removed it temporarily from the front of the nest. To determine whether defecation reduced the probability of a nestling being fed, we compared, for each brood, the average probability a nestling would be fed given that it just defecated, to the null hypothesis of a random distribution of feedings. A paired comparison t-test was used to compare the two sets of probabilities. The probability ofbeing fed following defecation was significantly lower than the average probability of a feeding (Table 4). (5) Are House Sparrow nestlings rejected? All three of the House Sparrow nestlings were fed by their foster parents, despite their different appearance, calls, and gaping behavior. Two reached weights (22.5 g and 29.0 g) consistent with expectations for normally fledged House Sparrows in the Lawrence area (Murphy 1977). On four occasions beginning on day seven at Nest 3 and on day 10 and 14 at one of the unmonitored nests, the House Sparrow nestling was found on the ground below the nest. Both nestlings were fed by their foster parents following replacement in the nest. Discussion. -Reed (198 1) concluded from his study of a single Song Sparrow brood that although feedings were randomly distributed among nestlings, a dominant nestling could receive a higher proportion of the feedings by maintaining a position in the nest near the parents arrival point. In his study, nestling position shifted in association with feeding bouts, and no nestling was dominant. Our observations showed that the total distribution of food items to nestling Barn Swallows was generally independent of nestling rank. High rank, however, increased the probability that a nestling would be fed, given that it gaped. In Nest 1, the feedings were not randomly distributed among the nestlings, and the difference was most pronounced at young nestling ages (Table 2). We found, unlike Reed ( 198 1 ), that the reordering of nestling position following defecation changed the probability that particular nestlings would be fed. Like Reed (1981), we feel

290 THE WILSON BULLETIN l Vol. 98, No. 2, June 1986 Year TABLE 4 Emc~ OF DEFECATION ON FEEDING PROBABLITIES Nest number Mean probability of bemg fed Probabiltty of being fed following defecation 1980 1 (46)b 0.33 0.22 2 (10) 0.33 0.10 3 (10) 0.17 0.10 1981C 4 (9) 0.25 0.22 6 (13) 0.25 0.15 a Average difference between probabilities is sgnificantly different from 0.0, d = 0.128, S, = 0.07, 1 = 4.08, df = 4, P < 0.05. b Number of observedefecations. C No defecations were observed at Nest 5. \ that the periodic change of nestling position could result in an even distribution of food to all nestlings in a brood if the nestlings are of about equal weight. The feeding of the House Sparrow nestlings implies that Barn Swallow parents cannot distinguish their own nestlings from those of a different species. Burtt (1977) showed that Barn Swallow parents distinguish their own offspring from other conspecific young only after fledging. Adults behave aggressively toward alien fledglings that are soliciting feedings. There is good evidence that in colonial swallows parental recognition of nestlings does not occur until chicks are mobile enough to leave the nest (Beecher et al. 1981a, b). Stoddard and Beecher (1983) suggesthat individual chick facial patterns may facilitate recognition in the Cliff Swallow. As our nestlings were individually marked for our recognition, they might have appeared distinctive to their parents. Our observations ended prior to fledging, however, and the distribution of feedings did not suggest any within-nest discrimination by parents. As parent Barn Swallows cannot distinguish among their nestlings, they are not capable of actively manipulating brood size following hatch. Brood-size changes are likely the result of differing competitive abilities among nestlings. Despite the general acceptance of this concept, studies of nestling feeding have found only a weak relationship between nestling size and proportion of feedings received (Best 1977, RydCn and Bengtsson 1980, Reed 1981). However, larger nestlings tend to receive a higher proportion of first feedings and need to gape less for food (Lohrl 1968, Ryden and Bengtsson 1980). In times of food shortage, larger nestlings may force the starvation of their smaller siblings, but this has yet to be demonstrated empirically. Acknowledgments. -We thank D. Kettle and S. C. McGillivray for initial preparation of the study site. J. Anderson, C. Brown, D. DeSteven, R. F. Johnston, P. E. Lowther, S. C..McGillivray, M. T. Murphy, R. J. O Connor, J. Rising, and N. A. Slade helped improve versions of this manuscript with constructive comments. Facilities on the Nelson Environmental Studies Area were provided by the Field Facilities Committee, University of Kansas. Thanks go to C. Steinhilber for her typing. LITERATURE CITED BEECHER, M. D., I. M. BEECHER, AND S. LUMPKIN. 198 la. Parent-offspring recognition in Bank Swallows (Riparia riparia): I. Natural history. Anim. Behav. 29:86-94.

GENERAL NOTES 291 -- AND S. HAHN. 1981b. Parent-offspring recognition in Bank Swallows (R igaria riiaria): II. Development and acoustic basis. Anim. Behav. 29:95-101. BEST, L. B. 1977. Patterns of feeding Field Sparrow young. Wilson Bull. 25166-187. BURST, E. H., JR. 1977. Some factors in the timing of parent-offspring recognition in swallows. Anim. Behav. 2523 l-239. CROSSNER, K. A. 1977. Natural selection and clutch size in the European Starling. Ecology 58:885-892. DAWKINS, R. 1976. The selfish gene. Oxford Univ. Press, New York, New York. HOWE, H. F. 1976. Egg size, hatching asynchrony, sex, and brood reduction in the Common Grackle. Ecology 57: 1195-1207. L~~HRL, H. 1968. Das Nesthakchen als biologisches Problem. J. fur Omithol. 109:383-395. MURPHY, E. C. 1977. Breeding ecology of House Sparrows. Ph.D. diss., Univ. Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas. O CONNOR, R. J. 1978a. Growth strategies in nestling passerines. Living Bird 16:209-238. -. 1978b. Brood reduction in birds: selection for fratricide, infanticide and suicide? Anim. Behav. 26:79-96. REED, J. R. 198 1. Song Sparrow rules for feeding nestlings. Auk 98:828-83 1. RICHTER, W. 1982. Hatching asynchrony: the nest failure hypothesis and brood reduction. Am. Nat. 120:828-832. -. 1984. Nestling survival and growth in the Yellow-headed Blackbird, Xanthocephalus xanthocephalus. Ecology 65:597-608. RICKLEFS, R. E. 1965. Brood reduction in the Curve-billed Thrasher. Condor 67:505-5 10. ROTHSTEIN, S. 1982. Successes and failures in avian egg and nestling recognition with comments on the utility of optimality reasoning. Am. Zool. 22:547-560. RYDI%N, 0. AND H. BENGTSSON. 1980. Differential begging and locomotory behaviour by early and late hatched nestlings affecting the distribution of food in asynchronously hatched broods of altricial birds. Z. Tierpsychol. 53:209-224. SKUTCH, A. F. 1976. Parent birds and their young. Univ. Texas Press, Austin, Texas. STINSON, C. H. 1979. On the selective advantage of fratricide in raptors. Evolution 33: 1219-1225. STODDARD, P. K. AND M. D. BEECHER. 1983. Parental recognition of offspring in the Cliff Swallow. Auk 100:795-799. WALPOLE, R. E. AND R. H. MYERS. 1978. Probability and statistics for engineers and scientists, 2nd ed., Macmillan, New York, New York. W. BRUCE MCGILLIVRAY AND HOWARD LEVENSON, Dept. Ornithology, Provincial Museum of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta T5N OM6, Canada; and O&e of Technology Assessment, Washington, D.C. 20510. Received 16 Jan. 1984, accepted 16 Oct. 1985. Wilson Bull., 98(2), 1986, pp. 291-296 Species-area relationship of migrants in isolated woodlots in east-central Illinois.-Many species of birds use woodlots temporarily for resting and foraging while on migration (Martin 1980, Graber and Graber 1983), and the loss of forest habitat and its fragmentation into isolated patches may adversely affect these birds (Graber and Graber 1983). Isolated patches of forest may be important particularly to migrants in highly disturbed landscapes where