ULTRAVIOLET REFLECTANCE OF COLORED PLASTIC LEG BANDS

Similar documents
Structural and melanin coloration indicate parental effort and reproductive success in male eastern bluebirds

IS THE ULTRAVIOLET WAVEBAND A SPECIAL COMMUNICATION CHANNEL IN AVIAN MATE CHOICE?

An experimental test of female choice relative to male structural coloration in eastern bluebirds

Blue structural coloration of male eastern bluebirds Sialia sialis predicts incubation provisioning to females

Achromatic plumage reflectance predicts reproductive success in male black-capped chickadees

Blue, not UV, plumage color is important in satin bowerbird Ptilonorhynchus violaceus display

The Evolution of Signal Design in Manakin Plumage Ornaments

SEXUAL SELECTION ON PLUMAGE COLOR IN A NORTH CAROLINA POPULATION OF EASTERN BLUEBIRDS. Callie Lynn Younginer. Honors Thesis

MA41 Colour variability and the ecological use of colour in the chameleons and geckos of Mahamavo

Preening, plumage reflectance and female choice in budgerigars

Reproductive success and symmetry in zebra finches

Everyday Mysteries: Why most male birds are more colorful than females

Plumage coloration can be perceived as a multiple condition-dependent signal by Great Tits Parus major

Assortative mating by multiple ornaments in northern cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis)

Pierre-Paul Bitton and Russell D. Dawson

Carotenoid-based plumage coloration and aggression during molt in male house finches

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Elk (Cervus canadensis) Extra credit assignment. Sad Underwing (Catocala maestosa) 10/11/2017

The evolutionary significance of plumage variability in

Effects of nestling condition on UV plumage traits in blue tits: an experimental approach

BLUE MALES AND GREEN FEMALES: SEXUAL DICHROMATISM IN THE BLUE DACNIS (DACNIS CAYANA) AND THE SWALLOW TANAGER (TERSINA VIRIDIS)

Sexy smells Featured scientist: Danielle Whittaker from Michigan State University

Carotenoid-based breast plumage colour, body condition and clutch size in red fodies (Foudia madagascariensis)

Cryptic sexual dichromatism occurs across multiple types of plumage in the Green-backed Tit Parus monticolus

EFFECTS OF COMMON ORIGIN AND COMMON ENVIRONMENT ON NESTLING PLUMAGE COLORATION IN THE GREAT TIT (PARUS MAJOR)

FEATURE ARTICLES SEXUAL DICHROMATISM, DIMORPHISM, AND CONDITION- DEPENDENT COLORATION IN BLUE-TAILED BEE-EATERS

PLUMAGE BRIGHTNESS AND BREEDING-SEASON DOMINANCE IN THE HOUSE FINCH: A NEGATIVELY CORRELATED HANDICAP? 1

Sexual selection based on egg colour: physiological models and egg discrimination experiments in a cavity-nesting bird

NATURAL AND SEXUAL VARIATION

Ultraviolet reflectance influences female preference for colourful males in the European serin

What Birds. Evolution has endowed birds with a system of color vision that surpasses that of all mammals, including humans

Sexual Dichromatism in the Blue-fronted Amazon Parrot (Amazona aestiva) Revealed by Multiple-angle Spectrometry

ECOL /8/2019. Why do birds have colorful plumage? Today s Outline. Evolution of Animal Form & Function. 1. Functions of Colorful Plumage

Distance and the presentation of visual stimuli to birds

Effect of feather abrasion on structural coloration in male eastern bluebirds Sialia sialis

Within-Male Melanin-Based Plumage and Bill Elaboration in Male House Sparrows

Back to basics - Accommodating birds in the laboratory setting

EGG SIZE AND LAYING SEQUENCE

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

Sexual imprinting on a novel blue ornament in zebra finches

Dacnis cayana (Blue Dacnis or Turquoise Honeycreeper)

Categorical perception of colour signals in a songbird

Variable visual habitats may influence the spread of colourful plumage across an avian hybrid zone

The Brain and Senses. Birds perceive the world differently than humans. Avian intelligence. Novel feeding behaviors

Supplementary Materials for

[ Post a Response Precious Fids Chat ] Novel Chemistry at Work To Provide Parrot's Vibrant Red Colors.

A case of achromatopsia. Perceptual Colour Space. Spectral Properties of Light. Subtractive Colour Mixture. Additive Colour Mixture

Perception & Attention Course. George Mather

Colour bands, dominance, and body mass regulation in male zebra nches (Taeniopygia guttata)

SEX DETERMINATION OF THE ACADIAN FLYCATCHER USING R. RANDY WILSON

PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE

Differences in Visual Signal Design and Detectability between Allopatric Populations of Anolis Lizards

Brightness variability in the white badge of the eagle owl Bubo bubo

doi: /

VALIDATING THE ASSUMPTIONS OF THE MAYFIELD METHOD

Plumage colour and feather pecking in laying hens, a chicken perspective?

How do low-quality females know they re low-quality and do they always prefer low-quality mates?

Color Vision: How Our Eyes Reflect Primate Evolution

Female zebra finches prefer unfamiliar males but not when watching noninteractive video

STATUS SIGNALING IN DARK-EYED JUNCOS

VARIATION IN PLUMAGE COLORATION OF EASTERN BLUEBIRDS (SIALIS SIALIS) IN RELATION TO WEATHER AND GEOGRAPHY. Mary-Catherine Warnock.

Plumage and its Function in birds

Plumage color as a dynamic trait: carotenoid pigmentation of male house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus) fades during the breeding season

Why do Anolis dewlaps glow? An analysis of a translucent visual signal

A CITIZEN S GUIDE TO IDENTIFYING AND CORRECTING PROBLEM LIGHTS ADJACENT TO SEA TURTLE NESTING BEACHES

Different colors reveal different information: how nutritional stress affects the expression of melanin- and structurally based ornamental plumage

Plumage Colours and the Eye of the Beholder

Cosmetic enhancement of signal coloration: experimental evidence in the house finch

DO BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS?

Procnias averano (Bearded Bellbird)

Computer Vision-Based Animal Preference Assessment Do Laying Hen Chicks Prefer Light with UVA Radiation?

Exercise 4: Animal Adaptations

THE DETECTION OF NEAR-ULTRAVIOLET LIGHT BY NONMIGRATORY AND MIGRATORY BIRDS JOHN W. PARRISH, JAMES A. PTACEK, AND KEVIN L. WILL

Volusia County Lighting Ordinance

AnOn. Behav., 1971, 19,

Winter male plumage coloration correlates with breeding status in a cooperative breeding species

THE NUMBER OF PROVISIONING VISITS BY HOUSE FINCHES PREDICTS THE MASS OF FOOD DELIVERED

Breeding Spangles by Ghalib Al-Nasser

EVIDENCE OF DELAYED PLUMAGE MATURATION AND CRYPTIC DIFFERENTIATION BETWEEN JUVENILES AND FEMALES

Ultraviolet reflectance and cryptic sexual dichromatism in the ocellated lizard, Lacerta (Timon) lepida (Squamata: Lacertidae)bij_

The number of visits to the nest by parents is an accurate measure of food delivered to nestlings in Tree Swallows

The Effect of Full-Spectrum Fluorescent Lighting on Reproductive Traits of Caged Turkey Hens 1 ' 2

Medical Genetics and Diagnosis Lab #3. Gel electrophoresis

STUDENT MANUAL CANINE SEARCH SPECIALIST TRAINING UNIT 8: ADVANCED RUBBLE SEARCH

PLUMAGE COLORATION OF THE BLUE GROSBEAK HAS NO DUAL FUNCTION: A TEST OF THE ARMAMENT ORNAMENT MODEL OF SEXUAL SELECTION

Melanin coloration in New World orioles II: ancestral state reconstruction reveals lability in the use of carotenoids and phaeomelanins

Color Vision by Prof/Faten zakareia King Saud University Physiology Dept

Hole-nesting birds. In natural conditions great and blue tits breed in holes that are made by e.g. woodpeckers

Trunk Contents. Crane Flight Feathers (3)

Fiery red heads: female dominance among head color morphs in the Gouldian finch

Red plumage and its association with reproductive success in red-capped robins

How blue are British tits? Sex, age and environmental effects

Physical and social environment for sheep

Low Cross-Sex Genetic Correlation in Carotenoid-Based Plumage Traits in the Blue Tit Nestlings (Cyanistes caeruleus)

Diet quality affects an attractive white plumage pattern in dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis)

NEST BUILDING IN HOUSE WRENS

CAROTENOID-BASED DEWLAP COLOR AS A VISUAL SIGNAL IN SOCIAL. COMMUNICATION OF BROWN ANOLES (Norops sagrei) John Edward Steffen

EDGER ADD DESIGN TO YOUR LIFESTYLE

Physiological, morphological and behavioural effects of selecting zebra finches for divergent levels of corticosterone

Bacteria as an Agent for Change in Structural Plumage Color: Correlational and Experimental Evidence

Feather mite abundance increases with uropygial gland size and plumage yellowness in Great Tits Parus major

Transcription:

J. Field Ornithol., 70(2):236-243 ULTRAVIOLET REFLECTANCE OF COLORED PLASTIC LEG BANDS K wrq J. McGP, W, GEOFFREY E. HILL, AND AMBER J. K se} 2 Department of Zoology and Wildlife Science and Alabama Agricultural Experiment Station 331 Funchess Hall Auburn University, Alabama 36849 USA Abstract.--The bright coloration of plastic leg bands used in avian research has been shown to affect the social interactions of birds by augmenting the ornamentation that traditionally mediates intersexual mate choice and/or intrasexual competition for mates. With ultraviolet (UV) vision and plumage reflectance playing a role in the mating decisions of some species, it would seem important to examine the UV reflectance properties of colored bands, as these bands may be providing strong and meaningful UV color signals that are invisible to the human eye and are potentially unaccounted for in most avian behavioral studies. In this study, we used a fiber optic spectrometer and a UV/visible light source to collect spectral reflectance data for the commonly used types and colors of bands and showed that none exhibit peak reflectance values throughout the range of UV wavelengths that is visible to birds. Instead, UV reflectance of colored bands at these wavelengths is comparable to levels of background reflectance found elsewhere in the avian visible spectrum. Consequently, colored bands lack discrete UV color signals and can safely be used in studies of wild birds with a minimal risk of influencing intraspecific signaling that is based on ultraviolet ornamentation. However, caution must be exercised when using these bands under experimental conditions, as certain treatments (e.g., indoor lighting, one-way glass, sunscreens) may mask the background levels of UV reflectance that still contribute to the overall band color, effectively inducing a color change in the bands that could provide unnatural visual cues to the birds and unintentionally influence their social interactions. REFLECTANCIA ULTRAVIOLETA DE LAS BANDAS PI_ STICAS DE COLORES PARA LAS PATAS. Sinopsis Se ha demostrado que la coloraci6n brillante de las bandas pl sticas para las patas usadas en investigaci6nes de aves afectan las interacciones sociales de estas al aumentar la ornamentaci6n que tradicionalmente media la selecci6n intersexual de parejas y/o la competencia intrasexual por parejas. Como la visi6n de luz ultravioleta (UV) y la reflectancia del plumaje juegan un rol en las decisiones de apareamiento de ciertas especies, pareceria importante examinar la reflectancia en UV de las bandas de colores, ya que podrian producir seriales fuertes y significativas en color UV que son invisibles al oho humano y que no son consideradas en la mayorca de los estudios eto16gicos. En este estudio usamos un espectr6- metro de fibra 6ptica y una fuente de luz visible/uv para obtener datos de reflectancia espectral para los colores y tipos de bandas comunmente usadas y mostramos que ninguna exhibe valores de reflectancia mayores a trav6s de los largos de onda UV que son visible a las aves. Por el contrario, la reflectancia de las bandas de colores en estos largos de onda son similares a los niveles de reflectancia del fondo hallados a trav6s del espectro visible de las aves. Consecuentemente, las bandas de colores carecen de seriales de color UV discreto y se pueden usar de forma segura en estudios de aves silvestres con un minimo de riesgo de influenciar las seriales intraspecificas basandos en ornamentaci6n ultravioleta. Sin embargo, se debe tener cuidado cuando se usen estas bandas bajo condiciones experimentales ya que ciertos tratamientos (e.g., luces interiores, espejos de visibilidad de un lado, cremas protectoras de pied pueden enmascarar los niveles de reflectancia de UV del fondo que todav a 1 Current address: Department of Neurobiology and Behavoir, Seeley G. Vludd Hall, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York 14853 USA. 2 Current address: Department of Genetics, Life Sciences Building, University of Georgia, Athens, Georgia 30602 USA. 236

Vol. 70, No. 2 UV Reflectance of Colored Bands [237 contribuye al color general de la banda, induciendo efectivamente un cambio en color en las bandas que podian producir sefias visuales nonaturales alas aves e influenciar no inten- cionalmente sus interacciones sociales. The classic means by which individual birds are identified in both field and captive studies is through the application of colored plastic leg bands (Spencer 1978). This technique assumes that colored bands do not significantly alter any aspect of avian social behavior. In some cases, it has been demonstrated that colored bands do not affect the interactions of birds (Watt 1982, Beletsky and Orians 1989, Belthoff and Gauthreaux 1991, Weatherhead et al. 1991, Cristol et al. 1992, Hannon and Eason 1995). In other studies, however, colored leg bands have been shown to influence a variety of bird behaviors, including mate choice (Burley et al. 1982, Burley 1986a,b; Johnson et al. 1993, Swaddle and Cuthill 1994, Bennett et al. 1996, Fiske and Amundsen 1997), mate guarding (Johnsen et al. 1997), and intrasexual competition (Metz and Weatherhead 1991). Thus, indiscriminant use of these bands could undermine many of the behavioral studies in this field. In fact, some researchers avoid using leg colors that are similar to the ornamental coloration of the bird under study. These concerns warrant further, careful consideration of the signaling properties of colored leg bands. To address this issue fully, we must consider not only colors in the visible spectrum (400-700 nm), but also those in the ultraviolet (UV) (290-400 nm). Given our inability to see UV wavelengths of light (Wald 1952), reflectance in the ultraviolet has rarely been considered. However, several avian species are known to possesspecialized UV-sensitive retinal cones (Chen et al. 1984, Chen and Goldsmith 1986) and associated oil droplets (Goldsmith et al. 1984) that extend their vision into the shortwavelength portion of the spectrum (Parrish et al. 1984). Peak visual sensitivity in this range appears to be between 350 and 380 nm (Emmerton and Delius 1980, Chen et al. 1984, Maier and Bowmaker 1993), with a lower limit of around 325 nm (Govardovskii and Zueva 1977). Additionally, the plumage of a number of species has been shown to reflect UV light (Burkhardt 1989, Burkhardt and Finger 1991, Maier 1993, Bleiweiss 1994, Andersson 1996, Bennett et al. 1996, 1997, Andersson and Amundsen 1997, Johnsen et al. 1997, Andersson et al. 1998, Keyser and Hill 1999), and ultraviolet vision and plumage reflectance can play a role in mate choice (Maier 1993, Bennett et al. 1996, 1997, Andersson and Amundsen 1997, Andersson et al. 1998, Hunt et al. 1998). Hence, it appears both valuable and necessary to characterize the degree of ultraviolet light reflectance for the colors and types of plastic leg bands used in avian research, as the bands may provide meaningful UV color signals to the birds that are invisible to humans and have subsequently gone unaccounted for in both field and captive studies. This issue has been partially addressed byjohnsen et al. (1997), who used spectroradiometry to quantify the UV reflectance of colored bands used in their study of wild Bluethroats (Luscinia svecica) and found low to moderate

238] K. J. McGraw et al. j. Field Ornithol. Spring 1999 reflectance levels in four colors of hobby beads. However, as of yet no one has investigated the UV signaling capacity of all of the bands used by field ornithologists. Here we used a fiber optic spectrometer and a UV/visible light source to provide spectral reflectance data for the colors and brands of plastic leg bands that are commonly used in avian studies. METHODS In search of color signals, we work under Andersson's (1996) assumption that "colourfulness... is related to deviations from uniform reflectance." The absence of color is typical of the two shades, black (no light reflectance) and white (near-perfect reflectance), that both show uniform reflectance. In contrast, colors are characterized by the presence of peaks at certain wavelengths; for example, an object is perceived to be blue when it exhibits peak reflectance at a wavelength between 425 and 475 nm. In this study, we looked for discrete reflectance peaks within the range of UV wavelengths known to be visible to birds (325-400 nm) and above typical background readings throughout the rest of the spectrum. The three most commonly used types of colored leg bands are hobby beads, split-ring bands, and wrap-around bands. Although hobby beads are manufactured by a number of companies, Perler Beads (Novacon Corp., Santa Rosa, California) are often used in avian research. These beads come in solid, translucent, fluorescent and glittery styles (Hill 1992) and fit most passerines and other species with similar sized tarsi. Wraparound bands from the National Band and Tag Company (Newport, Kentucky) and split-ring bands from A. C. Hughes (Hampton Hill, Middlesex, UK) have also been traditionally used. In our spectrometric analyses of the three band types, we have only included the most commonly used solid colors. We collected reflectance data with an Ocean Optics, Inc. S2000 fiber optic spectrometer (Dunedin, Florida). The unit is fitted with a grating that acts as a prism and divides light reflected from the surface into its spectral components from 250-880 nm. However, we only present data from 300-700 nm, which spans the range of UV and visible light wavelengths. The colored bands were illuminated simultaneously by a visible (tungsten-halogen) light source and an ultraviolet (deuterium) light source (Analytical Instrument Systems, Inc., Flemington, New Jersey). Readings were made with a reflectance probe that was held at a 90 ø angle to and 5 mm from the reading surface for all samples. A metal sheath mounted on the probe excluded all external light. Colored band readings were taken from an area 1.75 mm in diameter. Data integration time was set at 55 ms and optical pixel resolution was obtained using data smoothing level 10. Reflectance was measured as the percent of light reflected FIGURE 1. Reflectance spectra for the commonly-used colors of: a) hobby beads, b) wraparound bands, and c) split-ring bands. Each curve represents the average of 20 spectra.

-- Vol. 70, No. 2 UV Reflectance of Colored Bands [239 80 t - 70 30 20 lo 0 400 500 600 700 Wavelength (nm) lle. lea. B 90. 80. 70- _ yellow ørange'- 60. 50 blue 300 400 500 600 700 Wavelength (nm) lie lea 90. 80-70. 50. 30. 20-10 e- 400 500 600 700 Wavelength (nm)

240] K. j. McGraw et al. J. Field Ornithol. Spring 1999 from the surface of the band relative to a 99% Spectralon white reflectance standard. For each band, 20 spectra were measured and then averaged without lifting the probe tip from the band surface. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Reflectance spectra from the most commonly used colors and types of leg bands show that none of the bands exhibit discrete reflectance peaks in the ultraviolet portion of the spectrum (Fig. 1); thus, these bands lack hidden UV color signals. Instead, reflectance peaks are confined to the portion of the spectrum that is visible to humans. Interestingly enough, all colors and types of bands reflect some amount of UV light. This reflectance is often less than or equal to the baseline intensity at which light is reflected throughout the rest of the spectrum, and this passive reflectance at shorter wavelengths contributes to the overall, full-spectrum color that is exhibited by each band. These results are in agreement with those of Johnsen et al. (1997), who found a wider range of UV reflectance intensities among four colors of hobby beads, but also failed to detect substantial UV color peaks in the beads used in their study. Comparing the relative reflectance intensities of the band types, wraparound bands reflect more intensely than do split-ring bands, which in turn are more reflective than hobby beads (Fig. 1). Across all band types, light-colored bands (e.g., white, yellow) seem to exhibit more intense reflective peaks than darker colors (e.g., blue, green) (Fig. 1). The glossy plastic surfaces of certain bands are largely responsible for the generallyhigh reflectance values across the entire spectrum, and the differences in shininess among the three types are noticeable to the human observer (pers. obs.). Abrading the surfaces of the bands with sandpaper significantly reduces reflectance intensities across the entire spectrum, but does not change the overall shapes of the curves (pers. obs.). Colored bands differ both in the wavelengths of peak reflectance (the true color) and in the heights of the peaks (the reflectance intensity), and avian biologists can use the data presented here to weigh the benefits and risks associated with using bands of different color and manufacturer and select those bands that least influence the social system being studied, while maximizing band detectibility. Others may be interested in actually testing the influence of leg bands in a controlled situation, where matching plumage color with band color might be important. In this case, spectral data from feather patches can be compared to the colored band spectra we present here. Given the absence of UV reflectance peaks among the types and colors of plastic leg bands analyzed here, we now know that the use of these bands in avian studies of behavior in the wild should not disrupt intraspecific signaling systems based on ultraviolet ornamentation. However, it is imperative that we not ignore the UV reflectance that is in fact present in all of the colored bands examined. Although merely at background levels, the ultraviolet wavelengths of light reflected by the bands are an integral part of the overall color that is perceived by birds. Elimi-

Vol. 70, No. 2 UV Reflectance of Colored Bands [241 nating such reflectance would create a deviation in uniform reflectance, effectively inducing a color change that could influence the visual communication system under study. Conditions like these that mask background levels of UV reflectance are common in the experimental settings under which UV signaling in birds is currently investigated. In their studies of zebra finches, Bennett et al. (1996) used UV light filters to prevent the UV reflectance of plumage and colored bands from being transmitted to the signal receivers. Andersson and Amundsen (1997) have used human sunblock to prevent UV light from being reflected from body regions of male bluethroats, consequently preventing females from seeing areas that may reflect in the UV and from being able to use such plumage cues when selecting a mate. While these studies have employed such manipulations to explore the function of UV vision and plumage reflectance, any haphazard use of these techniques or others, in conjunction with colored bands, could unknowingly provide unnatural cues to the birds and influence the observed social interactions in an unintended fashion. In fact, Hunt et al. (1997) have emphasized the differences in the colored band preferences of zebra finches with respect to changing UV light conditions. Implicated in this scenario have been the use of one-way glass and mirrors that often absorb UV light (Swaddle and Cuthill 1994), as well as the weak emissions of UV light wavelengths by standard indoor lighting (Wysecki and Stiles 1967). This example highlights the need to carefully consider the experimental conditions to which study individuals are subjected when exploring the role of UV reflectance in social situations. As researchers become increasingly interested in the function of UV plumage ornamentation, the apparent challenge is to design controlled experiments that adequately manipulate UV signals that we cannot see. While our study demonstrates that biologists can continue to use colored bands, albeit more carefully, in avian research, we hope that it concurrently emphasizes the importance of UV vision in the behavioral interactions of birds. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Support for this work was provided by the National Science Foundation (Grant #IBN9722171 to G.E.H.), and by the College of Science and Mathematics, the Department of Zoology and Wildlife Science and the Alabama Agricultural Experiment Station at Auburn University. We extend a special thanks to the Hill lab and I. Cuthill for providing comments that improved the quality of the manuscript. LITERATURE CITED ANDERSSON, S. 1996. Bright ultraviolet colouration in the Asian whistling-thrushes (Myiophonus spp.). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 263:843-848., AND T. AMUNDSEN. 1997. Ultraviolet colour vision and ornamentation in bluethroats. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 264:1587-1591., I. ORNBORG, AND M. ANDERSSON. 1998. Ultraviolet sexual dimorphism and assortative mating in blue tits. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265:445-450. BELETSKY, L. D., AND a. H. ORIANS. 1989. Red bands and red-winged blackbirds. Condor 91: 993-995.

242] c. j. McGraw et al. J. Field Ornithol. Spring 1999 BELTHOFF, J. R., AND S. A. GAUTHREAUX, JR. 1991. Aggression and dominance in House Finchesß Condor 93:1010-1013. BENNETT, A. T. D., I. C. CUTHILL, J. c. PARTRIDGE, AND E.J. MAIER. 1996. Ultraviolet vision and mate choice in zebra finchesß Nature 380:433-435. --, --, --, AND K. LUNAU. 1997. Ultraviolet plumage colors predict mate preferences in starlings. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 94:8618-8621. BLEIV, ISS, R. 1994. Behavioural and evolutionary implications of ultraviolet reflectance by gorgets of sunangel hummingbirds. Anita. Behar. 48:978-981. BURKHARDT, D. 1989. UV vision: a bird's eye view of feathers. J. Comp. Physiol. A 164:787-796. --, AND E. FINGER. 1991. Black, white and UV: how birds see birds. Naturwiss. 78:279-280. BURLEY, N. 1986a. Sexual selection for aesthetic traits in species with biparental care. Am. Nat. 127:415-445. ß 1986b. Comparison of band colour preferences of two species of estrilid finches. Anita. Behav. 34:1732-1741. --, G. KRANTZBERG, AND P. RADMAN. 1982. Influence of colour-banding on the conspecific preferences of zebra finches. Anim. Behar. 30:444-455. CHEN, D., J. s. COLLINS, AND T. H. GOLDSMITH. 1984. The ultraviolet receptor in bird retinas. Science 225:337-340. --, AND T. H. GOLDSMITH. 1986. Four spectral classes of cone in the retinas of birds. J. Comp. Physiol. A 159:473-479. CRISTOL, D. A., C. S. CHIU, S. M. PECKHAM, AND j. F. STOLL. 1992. Color bands do not affect dominance status in captive flocks of wintering dark-eyed juncos. Condor 94:537-539. EMMERTON, J., AND J. D. DELlUS. 1980. Wavelength discrimination in the 'visible' and ultraviolet spectrum by pigeons. J. Comp. Physiol 141:47-52. FISKE, P., AND T. AMUNDSEN. 1997. Female bluethroats prefer males with symmetric colour bands. Anim. Behav. 54:81-87. GOLDSMITH, T. H., J. s. COLLINS, AND S. LICHT. 1984. The cone oil droplets of avian retinas. Vision Res. 24:1661-1671. GOVARDOVSIm, V. I., AND L. V. ZUEV^. 1977. Visual pigments of chicken and pigeon. Vision Res. 17:537-543. HANNON, S. J., mxd P. EASON. 1995. Colour bands, combs and coverable badges in willow ptarmigans. Anim. Behav. 49:53-62. HILL, G. E. 1992. An inexpensive source of colored leg bands. J. Field Ornithol. 63:408-410. HUNT, S., I. C. CUTHILL, J.P. SWADDLE, AND A. T D. BENNETT. 1997. Ultraviolet vision and band-colour preferences in female zebra finches, Taeniopygia gutrata. Anim. Behav. 54: 1383-1392., A. T. D. BENNETT, I. C. CUTHILL, AND R. GRIFFITHS. 1998. Blue tits are ultraviolet tits. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265:451-455. JOHNSEN, A., J. T. LIFJELD, AND P. A. ROHDE. 1997. Coloured leg bands affect male mateguarding behaviour in the bluethroat. Anim. Behav. 54:121-130. JOHNSON, K., R. DALTON, AND N. BURLEY. 1993. Preferences of female American Goldfinches (Carduelis tristis) for natural and artificial male traits. Behav. Ecol. 4:138-143. KES 3ER, A. J., AND G. E. HILL. 1999. Condition-dependent variation in the blue-ultraviolet coloration of a structurally based plumage ornament. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 266:771-778. MAIER, E.J. 1993. To deal with the "invisible": on the biological significance of ultraviolet sensitivity in birds. Naturwiss. 80:476-478., AND J. K. BOWMAKER. 1993. Colour vision in the passeriform bird, Leiothrix lutea: correlation of visual pigment absorbance and oil droplet transmission with spectral sensitivity. J. Comp. Physiol. A 172:295-301. METZ, K. J., AND P. J. WEATHERHEAD. 1991. Color bands function as secondary sexual traits in male red-winged blackbirds. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 28:23-27. PARRISH, J. w., J. A. PTACEK, AND K. L. WILL. 1984. The detection of near-ultraviolet light by non-migratory and migratory birds. Auk 101:53-58. SPENCER, R. 1978. Ringing and related durable methods of marking birds. Pp. 41-53, in B.

Vol. 70, No. 2 UV Reflectance of Colored Bands [243 Stonehouse, ed. Animal marking: recognition marking of animals in research. McMillan Press, London, United Kingdom. SW^DDI-E, J.P., ^,','D I. C. CUTHII_I_. 1994. Preference for symmetric males by female zebra finches. Nature 367:165-166. WALD, G. 1952. Alleged effects of the near ultra-violet on human vision. J. Opt. Soc. Amer. 42:171-177. WATT, D.J. 1982. Do birds use color bands in recognition of individuals? J. Field Ornithol. 53:177-179. WEATHERHEAD, P.J., D.J. HOYSAK, K.J. METZ, AND C. G. ECKERT. 1991. A retrospective analysis of red-band effects on red-winged blackbirds. Condor 93:1013-1016. WYSECI I, G., ) r ) W. S. S riles. 1967. Color Science. New York: John Wiley. Received 19 Dec. 1997; accepted 17 Jun. 1998.