Autumn territoriality of Chinese Grouse Bonasa sewerzowi at Lianhuashan Natural Reserve, Gansu, China

Similar documents
4B: The Pheasant Case: Handout. Case Three Ring-Necked Pheasants. Case materials: Case assignment

Lynx Update May 25, 2009 INTRODUCTION

Key concepts of Article 7(4): Version 2008

Long-eared Owl. For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Scott (1997) provides a comprehensive account.

Procnias averano (Bearded Bellbird)

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

Crotophaga major (Greater Ani)

By Hans Frey ¹ ² & Alex Llopis ²

Anhinga anhinga (Anhinga or Snake-bird)

Ecology and Management of Ruffed Grouse and American Woodcock

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Egg laying Late May to early June Mid-May to mid-july 3 to 10

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

Plumage and its Function in birds

6 Month Progress Report. Cape vulture captive breeding and release programme Magaliesberg Mountains, South Africa. VulPro NPO

(82) FIELD NOTES ON THE LITTLE GREBE.

Chris Knights and Terry Andrewartha have taken some remarkable. 440 [Brit. Birds 73: , October 1980]

Red-Tailed Hawk Buteo jamaicensis

Management of bold wolves

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

Population dynamics of small game. Pekka Helle Natural Resources Institute Finland Luke Oulu

Ames, IA Ames, IA (515)

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Chapman (1999) provides a comprehensive account.

Habitat Use and Survival of Gray Partridge Pairs in Bavaria, Germany

He was a year older than her and experienced in how to bring up a brood and survive.

Habitats provide food, water, and shelter which animals need to survive.

2009 Eagle Nest News from Duke Farms eagle nest Written by Larissa Smith, Assistant Biologist

Melanerpes rubricapillus (Red-crowned Woodpecker)

GREATER SAGE-GROUSE BROOD-REARING HABITAT MANIPULATION IN MOUNTAIN BIG SAGEBRUSH, USE OF TREATMENTS, AND REPRODUCTIVE ECOLOGY ON PARKER MOUNTAIN, UTAH

SOUTH-EASTERN LONG-EARED BAT, Nyctophilus corbeni. SQUIRREL GLIDER, Petaurus norfolcensis

Growth and Development. Embryonic development 2/22/2018. Timing of hatching. Hatching. Young birds and their parents

Woodcock: Your Essential Brief

( 162 ) SOME BREEDING-HABITS OF THE LAPWING.

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Clarke (1996) provides a comprehensive account.

Flight patterns of the European bustards

AS91603 Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants & animals to their external environment

NOTES ON THE SPRING TERRITORY OF THE BLACKBIRD

Key concepts of Article 7(4): Version 2008

Scholarship 2012 Biology

The breeding biology of endemic Spectacled Parrotbill (Sinosuthora conspicillatus) in Lianhuashan National Nature Reserve, Gansu Province, China

Bear Awareness Training

Sheikh Muhammad Abdur Rashid Population ecology and management of Water Monitors, Varanus salvator (Laurenti 1768) at Sungei Buloh Wetland Reserve,

The Greater Sage-grouse: Life History, Distribution, Status and Conservation in Nevada. Governor s Stakeholder Update Meeting January 18 th, 2012

Removal of Alaskan Bald Eagles for Translocation to Other States Michael J. Jacobson U.S Fish and Wildlife Service, Juneau, AK

The Animal World. Corner. Read the fact sheet and complete the exercise.

COLORADO LYNX DEN SITE HABITAT PROGRESS REPORT 2006

Parus major L. III. Home range of the basic flocks

PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE

AGGRESSIVE DISPLAY OF THE CORN-CRAKE.

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Laying May May 2 to 26. Incubation Early May to mid June Early May to mid June 30 to 34

Red panda research in Red Panda

ACTIVITY PATTERNS OF FEMALE RUFFED GROUSE DURING THE BREEDING SEASON

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Egg laying Late April to mid-may Mid-April to late June 5 to 12

State birds. A comparison of the Northern Mockingbird and the Western Meadowlark. By Shaden Jensen

INTRASPECIFIC AGONISM BETWEEN GIANT OTTER GROUPS. Carolina Ribas 1. Guilherme Mourão 2. Campo Grande, MS , Brazil. Brazil.

Please initial and date as your child has completely mastered reading each column.

Coyote and the Star LEVELED BOOK P. Visit for thousands of books and materials.

Original language: English CoP16 Prop. 16 CONVENTION ON INTERNATIONAL TRADE IN ENDANGERED SPECIES OF WILD FAUNA AND FLORA

Hawks Order Falconiformes

The Effect of Phase Shifts in the Day-Night Cycle on Pigeon Homing at Distances of Less than One Mile

Dacnis cayana (Blue Dacnis or Turquoise Honeycreeper)

Gallinaceous Birds Order Galliformes Family Phasianidae

From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research. Animal behavior then & now

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Gruiformes Family: Cariamidae Scientific Name: Cariama cristata Common Name: Red-legged seriema

Great Horned Owl (Bubo virginianus) Productivity and Home Range Characteristics in a Shortgrass Prairie. Rosemary A. Frank and R.

Back to basics - Accommodating birds in the laboratory setting

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

Monthly Review of the Cat & Dog Welfare Programmes, China & HK. Animals Asia Foundation. (March, 2012)

Ardea herodias (Great Blue Heron)

Introduction. Background. Reggie Horel Field Research 1st and 2nd hour June 3rd, Red Fox Telemetry

ECOLOGY OF ISOLATED INHABITING THE WILDCAT KNOLLS AND HORN

HEALTH, PRODUCTIVITY AND BEHAVIOUR OF CONVENTIONAL TURKEY BREEDS UNDER ECOLOGICAL OUTDOOR REARING CONDITIONS

The Development of Behavior

MANAGING AVIARY SYSTEMS TO ACHIEVE OPTIMAL RESULTS. TOPICS:

Transfer of the Family Platysternidae from Appendix II to Appendix I. Proponent: United States of America and Viet Nam. Ref. CoP16 Prop.

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Territorial signals. Design rules for territorial signals. Why defend a territory? Bird song and territory defense

Agenda. Warm-up: Look in your notebook for your grades. Review Notes on Genetic Variation Rat Island. Retake: Monday- last day!!!

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Site occupation and territorial display Early April Mid-March to early May

Proponent: Switzerland, as Depositary Government, at the request of the Animals Committee (prepared by New Zealand)

Bird-X Goose Chase / Bird Shield Testing Information For Use On: 1. Apples 2. Cherries 3. Grapes 4. Blueberries 5. Corn 6. Sunflowers 7.

BLACK BEARS, PEOPLE, AND GARBAGE DUMPS IN MINNESOTA

Raptor Ecology in the Thunder Basin of Northeast Wyoming

Coyote. Canis latrans. Other common names. Introduction. Physical Description and Anatomy. Eastern Coyote

Hole-nesting birds. In natural conditions great and blue tits breed in holes that are made by e.g. woodpeckers

48 RAPTOR RESEARCH Vol. 15 No. 2. top. < 0.Sm > side HABITUATION TO HUMAN DISTURBANCE IN NESTING ACCIPITERS

SHORT DESCRIPTION OF TECHNICAL PAPER CONTENT

Full house. the Burrowing Parrots of Patagonia. By JUAN F. MASELLO, CHRISTINA SOMMER and PETRA QUILLFELDT

Animal behaviour (2016, 2) THE SPOTTED HYENA

Internship Report: Raptor Conservation in Bulgaria

Breeding White Storks( Ciconia ciconia at Chessington World of Adventures Paul Wexler

Species must be adapted to their habitat.

WATTLED CRANE RECOVERY PROGRAMME Ensuring that Wattled Cranes continue to grace the skies and wetlands of South Africa

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Clarke (1995) provides a comprehensive account.

Breeding success of Greylag Geese on the Outer Hebrides, September 2016

Social Pattern and Behaviour Between Two Falco Species (Aves)

(130) DISPLAY OF THE MUTE SWAN

BIRDS OF NOVA SCOTIA ST. MARY S RIVER ASSOCIATION 2016

Snowshoe Hare. Lepus americanus. Other common names. Introduction. Physical Description and Anatomy. Snowshoe rabbit, varying hare, white rabbit

Dusky Grouse in Colorado: A Guide to Hunting and Viewing

OBSERVATIONS ON A PAIR OF NIGHTJARS AT THE NEST

Transcription:

Autumn territoriality of Chinese Grouse Bonasa sewerzowi at Lianhuashan Natural Reserve, Gansu, China SIEGFRIED KLAUS 1*, YUE-HUA SUN 2, YUN FANG 2 and WOLFGANG SCHERZINGER 3 1 Max Planck Institute of Biogeochemistry 7745 Jena, Hans Knöll-Str., Germany. 2 Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 8, P. R. China. 3 D-94568 St. Oswald, Guntherstr. 8, Germany. * Correspondence author - Siegi.Klaus@gmx.de Paper presented at the 4 th International Galliformes Symposium, 27, Chengdu, China. Abstract During 1995 to 27, 7 Chinese grouse (31 males and 39 females) were followed using radio telemetry at Lianhuashan Natural Reserve, Gansu Province, China. The typical habitat of Chinese grouse consisted of spruce-fir forests intermixed with thickets of many willow species (26 m to 35 m a.s.l.). The territorial behaviour of Chinese grouse culminated in October, as measured by whole-day registration of noisy drumming flights (dominant behaviour), flutter jumps (less frequent than in spring), and encounters of neighbouring males. Agonistic behaviour of males includes chasing each other or running in parallel, uttering two types of aggressive song, and standing in front of each other on the ground or in trees. Fighting was observed only in spring, but might occur in autumn too. Peaks of the diurnal territorial activities were found at 6-8 and at 15-18. The ecological consequences of autumn territoriality in comparison with hazel grouse are discussed. Keywords Bonasa sewerzowi, Chinese grouse, Lianhuashan reserve, territorial behaviour. Introduction The poorly known Chinese grouse Bonasa sewerzowi inhabits the coniferous forests in the high mountains of Gansu, Qinghai, Sichuan, Yunnan and Eastern Tibet Provinces, China. Whereas the territorial and mating behaviour of the Chinese grouse in spring has been described earlier (Klaus et al., 1996; Sun & Fang, 1997; Scherzinger et al., 23), their behaviour regarding the establishment and defence of territories in autumn has not yet been reported. From our radio telemetry studies, we have learned that both males and females establish territories in autumn and that both sexes form flocks in winter. Consequently, territorial activity is interrupted while they are in flocks. Studying a subpopulation of Chinese grouse, partially colour-ringed and wearing radio transmitters, we addressed the following questions: 1) which behavioural components are involved in territorial activities?; 2) are there differences in territorial behaviour in autumn and spring and in its diurnal distribution?; 3) is the pattern of foraging activity different in autumn and spring? Study area and methods Study area The study was conducted in the Lianhuashan Natural Reserve in Gansu Province, central China (34 o 56-58 N, 3 o 44-48 E). The highest peak is 3,578 m a.s.l. About 3% of the reserve (12,55 ha in total) is forested, but only 1,17 ha is mature coniferous forests growing on limestone-derived soils, mainly on northern slopes (Klaus et al., 21). The tree canopy is dominated by Abies fargesii, Picera asperata, P. purpurea, P. wilsonii, three species of Betula and many species of Salix. The optimal habitats of Chinese grouse are characterised by the close vicinity of coniferous forests with a shrub layer of different species (Sinarundinaria nitida, Berberis, Lonicera, Rhododendron, Rosa, Viburnum, Crategus, Spirea, Cotoneaster, Rubus etc.) that provides cover and groups of deciduous trees and shrubs (Salix spp., Betula spp., Sorbus spp., Hippophaae rhamnoides) that provide food in winter. The study area is described more thoroughly in Klaus et al., (1996; 21) and Sun et al., (23; 26). Methods Foraging and territorial activity of 18 colourringed and radio-tagged individuals was observed during field days in May 9-18, 1997 (with additional observations in May 27 for control, data not included in this work) and field days in October 13-22, 2 (n = 12 colour-ringed and radio-tagged males and females, with additional observations in October 26 for control, not included in this work). Entire-day observations concentrated on

Autumn territoriality of Chinese grouse 45 foraging males and females and the territorial activities of males (flutter jumps, noisy drumming flights, encounters). All types of behaviour (noisy drumming flights, flutter jumps and encounters of neighbouring males) seen or heard of a given number of birds observed per hour of the day were summed. In the case of foraging, the number of birds seen per hour along our fixed transect line (males and females combined) was summed. The relative values of all performances per hour of the day were obtained by setting the sum of these territorial behaviours observed during the -day period to %. The -day sum of any territorial performance was divided by the - day-sum of all performances and the resulting quotients were drawn as a function of day time. Times given are local times in China. In the description of the grouse behaviour, we follow the terminology of Hjorth (1965). The weather during both -day periods was comparable, varying between warm, sunny days and rain or even snow fall. The sum of the -day periods included the whole range both in May and October. The day length was different about 11 hours in October 2 (as an example: October 13: sunrise 7.12; sunset 18.34). The day length in May 1997 was 14 hours (as an example: May 9: sunrise 6.8, sunset 2.). This resulted in different length of grouse activity periods, but not in the shape of activity curves and in the relative proportions of the performances regarded here. Results The main element of territorial activity is the biphasic flutter jump, composed of a rising phase, a gliding and a landing phase (sonogram see Scherzinger et al., 26). The noisy drumming flight was recorded when the birds left or visited the roosting trees or when changing trees during feeding. Series of wing-beats were interrupted by gliding, producing acoustic signals. When neighbour males approached each other, conflicts followed. These were accompanied by two types of vocalizations, the aggressive song (sonogram see Klaus et al., 1996) and the trill song. Encounters were observed on the ground (more frequent) or in feeding trees or shrubs. During the encounter, the males started singing, often in duet, and climbed within the shrubs at distances of 1-5 m from each other. Chasing flights might follow. After a male visited a neighbour s territory, both rivals ran on the ground, one chasing the other or in parallel. When they interrupted the chase, they started to feed or crouch on the ground. During the entire encounter they uttered the aggressive or the trill songs (Klaus et al., 1998). The conflict culminated when males approached each other and stood in an erect pose in front of each other. Synchronous headbowing dominated this behaviour. Actual fights occurred rarely. They consisted of jumping into the air, pecking and loud wing-beating. Serious fights were observed only in spring, but they may occur in autumn too. In the early morning in autumn, after leaving the roost trees, the Chinese grouse fed in willows. In spring, flutter jumping started before foraging. In both autumn and spring, foraging prevails in the afternoon (67%) with peaks between 163 and 183 (or 193 in spring due to the prolonged daylight in May) (FIG. 1). The morning feeding period (4-12) was more extended in spring (6.3-12.3). In autumn, there was a more pronounced peak between 63 and 83. In both spring and autumn, 33% of feeding grouse were recorded in the morning (spring: n = 46, autumn: n = 59). Altogether, 143 foraging grouse were observed in spring and 177 in autumn. Activity of foraging (%) 25 2 15 5 spring % autumn % FIG. 1 Daily distribution of the relative frequency (%) of foraging Chinese grouse (males and females combined) in autumn and spring at Lianhuashan Natural Reserve, Gansu Province, China. The diurnal distribution pattern of territorial behaviours by males in autumn (% per hour of the day) is summarized in FIG. 2. In the morning, activities consisted of flutter jumps (14%, n = 28), noisy flights (3.5%, n = 6), and encounters (6%, n = 13). Before noon, only 23.5% (n = 47) of all territorial performances were recorded, with peaks around 83 and a resting time between 3 and 133. In the 28 World Pheasant Association.

46 Klaus et al. afternoon, the territorial activity was more pronounced: noisy flights dominated (41%, n = 81), followed by flutter jumps (2%, n = 39), and encounters (15%, n = 32). Overall, the dominating territorial activities were territorial flights (45%), flutter jumps (34%), and encounters (21%). Peaks of activity were recorded between 153 and 173, before intensive foraging took place (FIG. 1). In total, 2 territorial performances were registered in the -day period. Performances (%) 14 12 8 6 4 flutter jumps noisy flights encounters P <.1]). The mean length of the time interval between two successive flutter jumps of individual males also differed between spring and autumn, 2.5 ± 2. min (n = 75) and 21 ± 24 min (n = 4), respectively, (t = 5.23, d.f. = 42.2 P <.1). Foraging activity of Chinese grouse (both sexes combined) was concentrated during the second half of the day (67% of all observations of foraging grouse), with only small differences between spring and autumn. Comparing the peaks of the different activities in the morning, it was evident that males start their flutter jump display in spring before feeding. In contrast, foraging in autumn culminates before territorial activities started. 3 2 FIG. 2 Frequency distribution (%) over the day of three different forms (flutter jumps, noisy flights, encounters) of territorial behaviour exhibited by Chinese grouse males in October 2 registered during a -day period of pronounced activity at Lianhuashan Natural Reserve, Gansu Province, China. The diurnal distribution of territorial behaviour in spring differed from that in autumn. In spring, flutter jumps dominated, with the highest peak in the morning between 53 and 73 (73%, n = 467) and a smaller one in the afternoon (13%, n = 84) (FIG. 3). Noisy flights were much less frequent: 6% in the morning (n = 37) and 3% in the afternoon (n = 21). Encounters were registered in the morning (2.8%, n = 18) and in the afternoon (1%, n = 7). Overall, 82% (n = 522) of all territorial performances (n = 634 in the -day period) were recorded before noon and 18% (n = 112) in the afternoon. A global test showed a significant difference in the frequencies of flutter jumps, noisy flights and encounters between spring and autumn (χ 2 2 = 232.2, P <.1). Flutter jumps were the dominating territorial performance in spring (87% of all performances versus 34% in autumn [χ 1 2 = 228.7, P <.1]). In autumn noisy flights were dominant (45% vs. 9.3% in spring, χ 1 2 = 125.6, P <.1), followed by encounters (21% vs. 4% in spring [χ 2 2 = 7.2, Performances (%) 25 2 15 5 flutter jumps noisy flights encounters FIG. 3 Frequency distribution (%) over the day of three different forms (flutter jumps, noisy flights, aggressive encounters) of territorial behaviour exhibited by Chinese grouse males in May 1997 registered during a -day period of pronounced activity at Lianhuashan Natural Reserve, Gansu Province, China.

Autumn territoriality of Chinese grouse 47 Discussion In hazel grouse, both males and females defend year-round territories throughout most of their range, with peaks of activity in spring and autumn (Swenson, 1993; Bergmann et al., 1996). Territorial activity (whistling, flutterjumping, and rarely encounters) increases markedly just after the breaking-up of broods in early September - October, when yearlings start to find their own territories. At this time the birds respond to imitations of the territorial song with a hunter s whistle (Wiesner, 1977). In both autumn and spring, hazel grouse show peaks in response to the play-back (of the whistling song), but only in spring do they show a peak in spontaneous advertisement. Border disputes (encounters) were more common in autumn. In addition, responding hazel grouse males sing more often in spring than in autumn, when wing-produced sounds were more common (Swenson, 1991). In contrast, in both autumn and spring male Chinese grouse perform all three types of performances spontaneously. The activity pattern of Chinese grouse both in spring and in autumn was biphasic, with peaks in the morning between 63 and and in the afternoon between 15 and 18 (autumn), and peaks in the morning between 43 and 83 and in the afternoon between 15 and 2 (spring, FIGS. 1-3). Why do the males establish territories before the break-up of the territory system? The occupancy of a territory in autumn with a good supply of deciduous trees (winter food) and conifer cover is assumed to be crucial for the survival of both the hazel grouse and the Chinese grouse. In late October when frost and snow in Lianhuashan became common, the grouse entered flocks and went to the preferred winter habitats, first the females (mean 3.63 ±.77 km, n = 4) and then most of the males (mean 1.42 ±.44 km, n = 5). This resulted in a breakdown of the territorial system although some older males stayed in their territories in winter (Sun, 1997). In the northern and eastern parts of their range, hazel grouse also tend to form flocks composed of both sexes in late autumn and winter, which Swenson et al. (1995) interpreted to be an anti-predator behaviour that enables the bird to use additional food resources in more open habitats. As a consequence of flock formation, the territorial system established in autumn breaks down and is re-established in early spring. Autumn territoriality should benefit the territory owners, who should have the advantage of previous ownership when they return to their territories after the dissolution of flocks in spring. Most of the Chinese grouse in a local population join the flocks in a harsh winter with deep snow (unpublished results). The flocks over-wintered at places where food resources (buds and twigs of willows and berries of sea buckthorn) are most abundant. We suggest that flock formation in Chinese grouse, as in the hazel grouse, has evolved as a response to predation pressure in the open habitats, where the birds find a rich supply of food resources. Radio telemetry data showed that, after the resolution of flocks in late March to early April, first the males and later the females returned to their territories. We suggest that Chinese grouse select territories to provide rich food and cover for the early part of the reproduction period (pre-laying, laying and incubation). Swenson (1991) found that the hazel grouse, one of the smallest grouse, depends strongly on rich food in the pre-laying period, resulting in territoriality also in females. The Chinese grouse, which is the smallest of all the grouse (Sun et al., 24), lives in harsh high mountain habitats. We suggest that establishing a well defined territorial system in the autumn benefits the reproduction of territory holders in the following spring. Acknowledgements The work was supported by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) and Max- Planck-Gesellschaft (MPG), by the Chinese Academy of Sciences and by the National Natural Sciences Foundation of China (No. 362131). We are grateful to Hans-Heiner Bergmann, Stephen Browne and Jon E. Swenson for their comments on the manuscript and help with the statistics (by J. E. S.). References BERGMANN, H.-H., KLAUS, S., MÜLLER, F., SCHERZINGER, W., SWENSON, J.E. & WIESNER, J. (1996) Die Haselhühner. Magdeburg. HJORTH, I. (197) Reproductive behaviour in Tetraonidae. Viltrevy, 7, 181-596. KLAUS, S., SCHERZINGER, W. & SUN, Y.-H. (1996) Ökologie und verhalten des Chinahaselhuhns Bonasa sewerzowi. Der Ornithologische Beobachter, 93, 343-365. 28 World Pheasant Association.

48 Klaus et al. KLAUS, S., SCHERZINGER, W. & SUN, Y.-H. (1998) Territorial- und Werbeverhalten des Chinahaselhuhns (Bonasa sewerzowi). Journal für Ornithologie, 139, 185-186. KLAUS, S., SELSAM, P., SUN, Y.H., & FANG, Y. (21) Analyse von satellitenbildern zum schutz bedrohter arten. Fallbeispiel Chinahaselhuhn (Bonasa sewerzowi). Naturschutz und Landschaftsplanung, 33, 281-285. SCHERZINGER, W., S. KLAUS, SUN, Y.H. & FANG, Y. (26) Ethological and acoustical characters of the Chinese grouse (Bonasa sewerzowi) compared with sibling hazel grouse (B. bonasa) and ruffed grouse (B. umbellus). Acta Zoologica Sinica, 52 (Supplement), 293-297. SWENSON, J.E. (1991) Social organization of the hazel grouse and ecological factors influencing it. PhD thesis. University of Alberta. SWENSON, J.E. (1993) Hazel grouse (Bonasa bonasia) pairs during the non-breeding season: mutual benefit of a cooperative alliance. Behavioural Ecology, 4, 14-21. SWENSON, J.E., ANDREEV, A.V. AND DROVETSKIJ, S.V. (1995) Factors shaping winter social organization in hazel grouse Bonasa bonasia: a comparative study in the eastern and western palearctic. Journal of Avian Biology, 26, 4-12. SUN, Y.-H. (1997) Winter flocking behaviour of Chinese grouse Bonasa sewerzowi. Wildlife Biology, 3, 29. SUN, Y.-H., SWENSON, Y.E., FANG, Y., KLAUS, S. & SCHERZINGER, W. (23) Population ecology of the Chinese grouse in a fragmented landscape. Biology of Conservation, 1, 177-184. SUN, Y.-H., FANG, Y., SWENSON, J.E., KLAUS, S. & ZHENG, G.M. (24) Morphometries of the Chinese grouse Bonasa sewerzowi. Journal of Ornithology, 146, 24-26. SUN, Y.-H., KLAUS, S., FANG, Y., SELSAM, P. & JIA, C. X. (26) Habitat isolation and fragmentation of the Chinese grouse (Bonasa sewerzowi) at Lianhuashan Mountains, Gansu, China. Acta Zoologica Sinica, 52 (Supplement), 22-24. WIESNER, J., BERGMANN, H.-H., KLAUS, S., MÜLLER, F. (1977) Siedlungsdichte und habitatstruktur des haselhuhns (Bonasa bonasia) im Waldgebiet von Bialowieza Polen. Journal für Ornithologie, 118, 1-2. Biographical sketches SIEGFRIED KLAUS is active in both nature conservation and grouse research related to forest species in Central Europe and to rare endangered species in the Caucasus, Siberia and China. YUE-HUA SUN is Professor and Director of the Research Group on Avian Ecology studying evolution, behaviour, ecology and conservation of endemic birds in the forests around the Qinghai-Tibet Plateau. YUN FANG was a member of the research group, now undertaking a master s in wildlife biology, at the Norwegian University of Life Sciences. WOLFGANG SCHERZINGER is a zoologist within the Bavarian Forest National Park, specializing in nature conservation, behavior of owls, woodpeckers and grouse, and is involved in the Chinese grouse project.