The Role of Early Experience in the Development and Maintenance of Orientation Selectivity in the Cat's Visual Cortex: M. Stryker

Similar documents
Experimental analysis of amblyopia

Pre-natal construction of neural circuits (the highways are genetically specified):

preferring rightward movement. A changeover later than 5 weeks of age peak of the critical period for directional deprivation may occur earlier

THE POSTNATAL DEVELOPMENT OF THE VISUAL CORTEX AND THE INFLUENCE OF ENVIRONMENT

Differential Effects of Early Monocular Deprivation on Binocular and Monocular Segments of Cat Striate Cortex

deprived eye (reverse occlusion). beyond 1 year of age; only two of six animals recovered sufficient vision to enable

Binocular Impulse Blockade Prevents the Formation of Ocular Dominance Columns in Cat Visual Cortex

M. uch interest has recently been focused. Visual development in cats. 394 Pettigrew Investigative Ophthalmology. S.

Consequences of alternating monocular deprivation on eye alignment and convergence in cats. Randolph Blake, M. L. ]. Crawford, and Helmut V. B.

Effects of Early Monocular Lid Suture on Spatial and Temporal Sensitivity of Neurons in Dorsal Lateral Geniculate Nucleus of the Cat

Cortical Cell Orientation Selectivity Fails to Develop in the Absence of ON-Center Retinal Ganglion Cell Activity

spider monkeys by recording extracellularly from single units and stimulating

abnormal lateral geniculate body. His anatomical study suggested that chiasm instead of remaining uncrossed. They thus reach the wrong hemispheres,

injected eve. (Received 1 November 1977) with electrolytic lesions. A good correspondence was found between the location of

The Critical Period for Ocular Dominance Plasticity in the Ferret s Visual Cortex

Do blue-eyed white cats have normal or abnormal retinofugal pathways? R. W. Guillery, T. L. Hickey, and P. D. Spear

Binocular Interactions in Striate Cortical Neurons of Cats Reared with Discordant Visual Inputs

PATTERN EVOKED RESPONSE DEFICIENCY IN PATTERN DEPRIVED CATS 1

Binocular Exposure causes Suppression of the Less Experienced Eye in Cats Previously Reared with Unequal Alternating Monocular Exposure

Area Centralis Position Relative to the Optic Disc Projection in Kittens as o Function of Age

Animal Behavior: Biology 3401 Laboratory 4: Social behaviour of young domestic chickens

David H. Hubel. A Biographical Memoir by Robert H. Wurtz

Rapid Anatomical Plasticity of Horizontal Connections in the Developing Visual Cortex

Serendipity and the Siamese Cat: The Discovery That Genes for Coat and Eye Pigment Affect the Brain. Jon H. Kaas

THE PRETRIGEMINAL CAT AS AN INSTRUMENT FOR INVESTIGATION OF THE OCULAR FIXATION REFLEX

Development of Neuronal Response Properties in the Cat Dorsal Lateral Geniculate Nucleus During Monocular

The Development of Behavior

IIEIIIEEEEEEII. EE IEEEEI h. IihhEEEEE

CLARSBISHOP AREA IN THE CAT: LOCATION AIVD RETINOTOPICAL PROJECTION

Expression of a Surface-Associated Antigen on Y-Cells in the Cat Lateral Geniculate Nucleus Is Regulated by Visual Experience

Effects of Convergent Strabismus on the Development of Physiologically Identified Retinogeniculate Axons ih Cats

My recollections of Hubel and Wiesel and a brief review of functional circuitry in the visual pathway

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

geniculate nucleus of kittens raised with convergent squint in one eye,

Morphology of Retinogeniculate X and Y Axon Arbors in Cats Raised With Binocular Lid Suture

THE JOURNAL OF COMPARATIVE NEUROLOGY 233: (1985)

Neuroscience Letters

Parallel Processing in the Visual System THE CLASSIFICATION OF RETINAL GANGLION CELLS AND ITS IMPACT ON THE NEUROBIOLOGY OF VISION

Spatial and Temporal Sensitivity of Normal and Amblyopic Cats

Temperature Gradient in the Egg-Laying Activities of the Queen Bee

Emergence of Ocular Dominance Columns in Cat Visual Cortex by 2 Weeks of Age

lowering of the visual acuity. When closure was extended through the first by varying the age at eye closure. Waiting until 1 month of age

Analysis of Sampling Technique Used to Investigate Matching of Dorsal Coloration of Pacific Tree Frogs Hyla regilla with Substrate Color

Spatial Analysis of Ocular Dominance Patterns in Monocularly Deprived Cats

Sampling and Experimental Design David Ferris, noblestatman.com

PIGEON DISCRIMINATION OF PAINTINGS 1

Laminar and Columnar Distribution of Geniculo-cortical Fibers in the Macaque Monkey

1.4. Initial training shall include sufficient obedience training to ensure the canine will operate effectively based on mission requirements.

Dog Behavior Problems Veterinary Visits/Examinations

Regional Variation in the Representation of the Visual Field in the Visual Cortex of the Siamese Cat

(Received 22 November 1984) studies were made on twenty such pairs; eight X on-centre, seven Y on-centre, two

1Ila and V. Canberra, A.C.T. 2601, Australia (Received 21 March 1979)

Reversing Category Exclusivities in Infant Perceptual Categorization: Simulations and Data

1.5 C: Role of the Environment in Evolution Quiz

Effects of Retinal Image Degradation on Ocular Growth in Cats

[Frontiers in Bioscience 13, , May 1, 2008] Binocular phasic coactivation does not prevent ocular dominance segregation

Early Exploration of the Visual Cortex

Horizontal Interactions in Cat Striate Cortex: 111. Receptive Fields and Transient Exuberance of Tangential Interactions

Puppy Socialization and Fear Prevention

Plasticity in primary somatosensory cortex resulting from environmentally enriched stimulation and sensory discrimination training

EVOLUTION OF IDEAS ON THE PRIMARY VISUAL CORTEX, : A BIASED HISTORICAL ACCOUNT

Proceedings of the European Veterinary Conference Voorjaarsdagen

Visual and Instrumental Evaluation of Mottling and Striping

RETINITIS PIGMENTOSA*

Behavior Modification Why Punishment Should Be Avoided

Adjustment Factors in NSIP 1

EVOLUTIONARY GENETICS (Genome 453) Midterm Exam Name KEY

UTILITY OF THE NEUROLOGICAL EXAMINATION IN RATS

Approximating the position of a hidden agent in a graph

1 - Black 2 Gold (Light) 3 - Gold. 4 - Gold (Rich Red) 5 - Black and Tan (Light gold) 6 - Black and Tan

Pill Bug Environmental Preferences Based on Moisture

Effects of Feedback Projections From Area 18 Layers 2/3 to Area 17 Layers 2/3 in the Cat Visual Cortex

The contralateral impairment of the orienting ocular-following reflex after lesions of the lateral suprasylvian cortex in cats

log no. VNS23011 Ocular dominance columns in strabismus VNS23~6! :31 pm

Inhibitory mechanisms in the LGN: A possible substrate for amblyopia?

Rules of Connectivity between Geniculate Cells and Simple Cells in Cat Primary Visual Cortex

SELECTION FOR AN INVARIANT CHARACTER, VIBRISSA NUMBER, IN THE HOUSE MOUSE. IV. PROBIT ANALYSIS

Report. Darkness Alters Maturation of Visual Cortex and Promotes Fast Recovery from Monocular Deprivation

FOOTEDNESS IN DOMESTIC PIGEONS

ESTIMATING NEST SUCCESS: WHEN MAYFIELD WINS DOUGLAS H. JOHNSON AND TERRY L. SHAFFER

The Inheritance of Coat Colour in the Cardigan Welsh Corgi by Ken Linacre

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

Dog Behavior and Training - Moving with Your Dog

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production

Evolution in Action: Graphing and Statistics

Columnar Specificity of Intrinsic Horizontal and Corticocortical Connections in Cat Visual Cortex

AGGRESSIVE DISPLAY OF THE CORN-CRAKE.

1250 Reports. Axial lengths and refractive errors in kittens reared with an optically induced anisometropia. EARL L. SMITH, III, GREGORY W.

Shelter Guidelines Project. Shelter Guidelines - Content

7. Flock book and computer registration and selection

E erimental Brain Research 9 Springer-Verlag 1986

Distribution Unlimited

What causes lizards brains to change size?

TECHNICAL BULLETIN Claude Toudic Broiler Specialist June 2006

Key considerations in the breeding of macaques and marmosets for scientific purposes

This article is downloaded from.

Desensitization and Counter Conditioning

Distance and the presentation of visual stimuli to birds

Behavior Modification Reinforcement and Rewards

Transcription:

Reprinted from Neurosciences Research \rogram Bulleti~ VOl~e IS, Number 3, Neuronal mechan1sms 1n visual perception E. P~p~el, R. Held & J.E. Dowling, edito;s (Cambr1dge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1977) Pages 454-462 The Role of Early Experience in the Development and Maintenance of Orientation Selectivity in the Cat's Visual Cortex: M. Stryker The role that early visual experience plays in the development of the selective response properties of cells in the visual cortex has been investigated ex tensively. Two prominent organizing properties of the visual cortex are the selectivity of the cells for the orientation of a bar or edge in visual stimulus and the propensity of the cells to respond to stimuli presented through one eye or the other (Hubel and Wiesel, 1974a,b, and Bubel, above, "Architecture of the Monkey Striate Cortex"). A discussion of the role of early visual experience in determining the eye preference of cortical cells is presen ted elsewhere (Hubel et ai., 1976, 1977a; Wiesel, above, "Effects of Visual Deprivation on

Neurosciences Res. Prog. Bull., Vol. IS, No.3 455 Macaque Monkey Striate Cortex"). Stryker's report concerned the selectivity of cortical cells for edge orientation, offering an interpretation of experimental findings on the role of visual experience in the developmen t and main tenance of this selectivity. The most obvious experiment on the role of early experience is to examine cells of the visual cortex before the animal has acquired any visual experience. If the kinds of selectivity found in the adult animal are present. then experience is not required for their normal development. For the monkey the results of this experiment gave a conclusive answer: The cells of the newborn monkey's visual cortex n:sponded with selectivity indistinguishable from that of the allult for the orientation of a bar or edge visual pattern (Wiesel and Hubel, 1974). Furthermore, the orderly arrangement of cells in the cortex according to preferred orientation was also present in the newborn monkey (see Wiesel, above). Hence visual experience is not necessary to guide or otherwise promote the establishment of the neural connections underlying the orientation-selective response properties that are evident in the adult visual cortex. However, this experiment leaves open questions regarding the extent to which these connections may remain plastic or modifiable by visual experience. The cat is born with its visual system in a more immature state than that of the monkey; for the first week or more its eyes remain dosed. and its originally cloudy optic media do not allow a clear image to he focused on the retina until the animal is 2 to 3 weeks of age (Thorn et ai., 1(76). It might be thought that this later development of the visual system would allow a greater role for e~!rly visual experience in the eat's normal development than was found for the monkey; but the immaturity of the optical parts of the young kitten's visual system and the generally fragile condition of these animals make the m(' obvious experiment, examining the kitten before visual experie normally acquired, difficult to perform and interpret. An aiter" experiment is to allow the kitten to grow until it is several months OJ while deprived of visual experience by being reared in darkness or witil sutured eyelids. This experiment, though easier to perform, gave a clear result but an equivocal answer to our question. Most of the cells studied in such anim;.i1s were found not to be selective for orientation: rather they were unresponsive or very poorly re~ponsive to visual stimulii (Wiesel and Hubel, 1965b; Cynader et ai., 1976). It was not clear from these resuits whether the cells had failed to develop normal selectivity or whether an onginal :nnate selectivity had deteriorated as a consequence of this long period of deprivation. Visual experience was neces-

456 Neuronal Mechanisms in Visual Perception sary, but wh.:ther to IlIJintJiil Jil innj!e selectivity or to guide the developmen t of selectivity was unresolved. Thus. the experiment of looking at the very young. visually inexperienced animal required interpretation. Hubel and Wiesel (1963a) published the results of the first such experiment. Among the cells studied, they found examples of both simple and complex receptive fields with orientation selectivity. Some of the other cells responded abnormally. but this could have been due to abnormal recording cond i tions or poor optics. Barlow and Pettigrew (19 7 1: Pettigrew, 1974), disputed these findings. In a group of visually inexperienced animals they studied, almost none of the cells showed selectivity for stimulus orientation. These findings were lent additional weight through the use of quantitative methods. For some cells. the investigators counted the number of spikes evoked by stimuli of different shapes and orientations and made graphs showing response as a function of stimulus orien tation. Shortly thereafter, other workers reported similar resuits (Blakemore and Mitchell, 1973; Imbert and Buisseret. 1975). An experiment on the rolc of specific kinds of early experience in the development of the visual cortex was reported at about the same time. Hirsch and Spinelli (1970. 1971) raised kittens with extremely restrickd \'i~ual experience. Kittens wore opaque goggles whenever they were not in total darkness. Within the goggles. each eye of the kittens could see stripes of only one orientation, horizontal for onc eye and vertical for the other eye. liir~ch and Spinclli studied the visual cortex of these animals u<;ing a novel automated method, the findings of which are somewhat difficult to relate to those of conventional receptive field plotting by hand. They found, however, that two-thirds of the cells studied did not appear to be selective for stimulus orientation. Most of the remaining selective cells were monocularly driven and responded optimally to the orientjtion to which the dfeetive eye had been exposed. Two possible interpretations were offered for these results: (I) cells in the visual cortex were programmed to develop a preferred orientation according to the prominent features of the early visual environment (under this interpretation no particular explanation is ofkred for the two-thirds of the cells not sdective for orientation): or (2) only the cells that received appropriate stimulation early in life could maintain their innate orien tation seh:ctivity; cells not stimulated at their innate, preferred orientation became unresponsive or nonselective. The finding of Hubel and Wiesel (l963b) that many or most cells had an innate, preferred orientation, together with the results of many workers (Hubel and Wiesd, 1965: Cynader et ai., 1976) that eom-

Neurosciences Res. Prog. null., Vol. 15. No.3 457 pi de pattern deprivation early in life causes most cells of the cortex to lose selectivity for orientation, favored the second alternative above. The work of Barlow and Pettigrew (1971) and other similar reports (Blakemore and Mitchell, 1973; Pettigrew, 1974; 1m bert and Buisseret, (975) favored the first alternative, that cortical cells may be programmed during normal development to acquire selectivity for the redundant features of the early visual environment. Reports by Blakemore and co-workers (Blakemore and Cooper, 1970: Blakemore and Mitchell, 1973; Blakemore, (974) supported the first alternative. They raised kittens with a somewhat less severe but perhaps more natural restriction of early visual experience by allowing them to se..: only when standing on a glass plate inside a vertically or horizontally striped drum and wearing a ruff collar that obscured view of the body. Most cortical cells studied were selective for orientations similar to that to which the kittens had been exposed. These workers did not note the presence of large numbers of unresponsive or nonselective cells. Thus, these results suggested that all cortical cells can develop selectivity for the orientations present in early visual experience and that, if they have any innate selectivity, this must be modifiable. Barlow (1975 and tilis Work Session) offered an analogy between this second alternative scheme for cortical development and the phenomenon of imprinting in birds: Because of the difficulty of innately specifying the exact appearance of its mother, a baby duckling does not receive this information innately. Instead, it is innately programmed to follow and treat as its mother the most prominent large, noisy, moving object it encounters after hatching, allowing it the opportunity to learn its mother's appearance gradually, through experience. It was proposed that in a similar fashion the innate developmental program for the visual cortex might not be sufficiently detailed to specify the preferred orientation for each cell. Instead, each cell (or column of cells) might be programmed to use early visual experience with oriented edges to reinforce initially unspecified and random response biases. Thus it was proposed that just as all ducklings can be imprinted by a beach ball if their early experience is restricted to beach balls, so might all cortical cells become selective for the vertical orientation if early experience were restricted to vertical stripes rather than to a nonnal environment rich in orientations. This analogy incorporates an attractive teleological explanation for why orientation selectivity in the visual cortex should be modifiable by early experience. Any abnormal physiological state associated with the trauma of microciectrode recording might conceivably cause innately selective

458 Neuronal Mechanisms in Visual Perception cells in the fragile cortex of young kittens to respond nonselectively, whereas it seems unlikely that cells might appear to respond selectively when they were in fact innately nonselective. Hence the reports showing no orientation selectivity in visually inexperienced kittens (Barlow and Pettigrew. 1971; Pettigrew, 1974) were not entirely persuasive when compared with the report of selectivity in such animals (Hubel and Wiesel, 1963a), even though the former studies used quantitative methods to study some cells. For this reason, Sherk and Stryker (1976) investigated the orientation selectivity of cells in visually inexperienced kittens using an automated, quantitative procedure for assessment of each cell encountered. One of their findings (shown in Figure 71) is from a kitten that had been dark-reared from birth except for a brief KITTEN 2 DARK-REARED k2 LVCl Ul R E. TIITRLS 2' DRYS O~~ 8~1Z(o w (/) z o (l. (/) w a:: STIMULUS ORIENTATION Fi~urc 71. Photograph of urientation tunin!! histo~ram displayed 0<1 CRT sneen for the first cell Ievunj~d in a 26 dj} old dark reared kitten. The kilten\ eyes were opened for Ihe firsl time ImIlledlately prill! 10 compiling this histo!!iam. I"tal number of spikes lorjinale) arc plutted as a function of slunlllus oiientation (ahscissa); slllnulus was a bai u, light that,wept aero" the rccepti'e field. Ltch ui the 12 stimulus oiientations tested appeared a total of five times, in random order c vcpt that 110 presentation of anyone orientation Was repeated more than twice in a row. (f un the ab"'issa Iepresenh a vertical bar moving from right to left; 90 rep,c>enh a horizontal bar moving down; 180 0 represents a vertical bar moving from len to right, etc. I Str) kei I

Neurosciences Res. Prog. Bull., Vol. 15, No.3 459 period prior to the time of eye opening when its eyelids were sutured shut. At 3Y2 weeks of age, the kitten was removed from the dark and prepared for. microelectrode recordings; its eyelids were opened less than 2 min prior to compiling the histogram illustrated. The selectivity shown by such a cell is unlikely to be due to only 2 min of visual experience. Most (more than 80%) of the cells in these visually inexperienced animals were selective for the orientation of a moving bar or edge stimulus. In addition. preferred orientation changed gradually and progressively across the cortical surface as the electrode moved from cell to cell. This arrangement of the cells in the cortex according 0 preferred orientation, characteristic of the adult anim:ll, was striking in these visually inexperienced kittens. There are other reports of varying but considerable proportions of cells in similarly visually inexperienced kittens showing selectivity for orientation (Blakemore and Van Sluyters, 1974), confinning the earliest finding that, for many or most of the cells in the eat's visual cortex, orientation selectivity is innate (Hubel and Wiesel, 1963a). If orientation selectivity is innate, the experiments on kittens reared in striped drums suggesteu that it must :llso be modifiable by visual experience (Blakemore, 1974). Stryker and Sherk (1975) repeated these experiments using a similar rearing procedure but different recording techniques. Employing a systematic sampling technique designed to examine the distribution of orimtation preference across wide areas of the cortical surface, together with an automated assessment of preferred orientation for CJch cell studied and a blind procedure to guard against the effects of inadvertent experimenter bias, they did not find a biased distribution of preferred orientations in the visual cortex of the kittens. Instead, these kittens were like nonnal animals, with all orientations represented in the visual cortex and with preferred orient:' tion changing gradualiy and progressively across the cortical surfa, This finding is perhaps not surprising because the rearing procedure does not completely restrict kittens' visual experience to the orientation of the stripes within the drum. The animals may sometimes turn their heads from side to side or look up and down; and when they do this, the stripes may appear at many different orientations on their retina. Fitting kittens with striped goggles does restrict their visual experience almost completely to contours of just one orientation (Hirsch and Spinelli, 1970, 1971). The recording techniques described above (Stry ker ami Sherk, 1975), from which the find ings are c1o~cly comparable to thos,~ of conventional hand-plotting of receptive fields, were

460 Neuronal Mechanisms in Visual Perception abk to demonstrate the main effects reported in such kittens. When the goggles showed horizontal stripes to one eye ailli verticai stripes to the other. most of the cells became quite abnormal in that they were unresponsive or nonselective for orientation (Stryker and Sherk, 1975). "lost of the remaining selective cells had peferred ori..;ntations near horizontal or vertical and were monocularly driven by the eye whose e:nly experience was nearer the cell's preferred orientation. These results may be explained by theorizing that the role of early experience is only to main tain the responsiveness and selectivity of cells. each of which already has an innate sekctivity for a particular orientation. The results would come about as follows: Cells whose innate preferred orientations wen: oblique would not be stimulated by the pattern shown to either eye. These cells would effectively be binocularly deprived, somewhat like all cells in cats raised in the dark or with both eyelids sutured shut. In these latter circumstances, it is known that most of the cells would lose their responsivene~s or selectivity (Bubel and Wiesel, 1965: Cynader d ai., 197G). For the cells whose innate, preferred orientations wae near horizontal or vertical, a different known mechanism, similar to that of monocular deprivation, would come into pl:jy. The eye that experienced vertical would tend to take over all the cells whose preferred orientations were near vertical because these cells, lih all the cells in the cortex of a monocularly eyelidsullired cat, are stimulated by only one eye. The same mechanism "vuld cause the cells whose preferred orientations were ncar horizontal to come to be driven exclusively by the eye that had seen horizontal stripes in early life. Such an orientation-selective shift in eye dominance is known both in the cat (Cynader and Chernenko, 1976*) and in the monkey (Wiesel, 1977): When animals were reared viewing vertical stripes through one eye and either all orientations (in the cat) or nothing (in the monkey) through the other eye, cells selective for the orientations seen by both eyes or by neither eye were shared equally between the two eyes, while most of the cells selective for the orientation seen by only one eye were taken over by that eye. Thus, for the cats discussed above reared with one eye viewing horizontal and the other eye viewing vertical, mechanisms like those of two known phenomena, the one similar to that of binocular deprivation for the cells preferring obliques, and the other similar to that of monocular deprivation for the cells preferring horizontal or vertical. together account for the results obtained. There is no need to postulate some unknown new Also, personal communication.

Neurosciences Res. Prog. BuU., Vol. 15, No.3 461 mechanism by which the preferred orientation of a cortical cell can be modified by experience. Other recent results are also consiskilt with the theory that the role of eatly visual experience is only to maintain the responsiveness ~md selectivity of cells already selective for an innately preferred orientation. When kittens are reared wearing striped goggles that show the same orientation to both eyes, the theory would predict that a larger fraction of the cells should effectively be binocularly deprived. and hence become unresponsive or nonselective, than was the case when the goggles showed different orientations to the two eyes. This prediction has recently been confirmed. * Blasdel. Mitchell, Muir, and Pettigrew (1977) raised kittens that were allowed their only visual experience while their heads were fixed in place within a striped drum so as to restrict visual experience in a manner intermediate in severity and effectiveness between having the kittens wear striped goggles and allowing them to walk around in the striped drum. Almost a third of the cells studied were not selective for orientation, and among the remaining selective cells there was a bias toward the orientation to which the animal had been exposed. These results are completely consistent with those above, if the nonselective cells are assumed to be those whose original innately preferred orientations were different from the orientation prominent in the animals' early visual expericdce. Other theories proposing more elaborate roles for early visual experience than mere maintenance of innate selectivity, i.e.. that some cells may be innately specified while others are not or that some may be modifiable even if originally selective while others remain rigid, may also be made consistent with the experimental findings. However, current results do not seem to force us to concede a role for visual exper' ence in guiding the normal development of orientation selectivity in cat. A role for visual experience in only the maintenance of selectivll J is all that is required to explain the results in the cat and is consistent wi th the findings in the monkey. Teleological considerations do make a more extensive role for visual experience in the development of the visual cortex extremely attractive. For example. it is difficult to imagine an innate mechanism for binocular cells by which the rc(;cptive fields of the two eyes are made to match as closely as they do in the normal adult animal. A mechanism *M. Stryker, H. Sherk, Il.V.B. Hirsch, and A.G. Leventhal, Orientation Selectivity in Cat's Visual Cortex Followin~ Restricted Early Visual Experience Using Goggles: A Quantitative Study, manu>cript in press.

462 Neuronal Mechanisms in Visual Perception by which connections from the two eyes to such a cell are modified by visual experience, strengthened when the activity of the cell and its inputs are correlated and weakened by passive decay or uncorrelated activity, can easily account for the similarity of the receptive fields of the two eyes (Hebb, 1949; Marisberg, 1973; Blakemore and van Sluyters, 1974; Nass and Cooper, 1975). But some cells in Siamese cats with "mirror fields" (such a cell has two separate receptive fields in different retinal locations of the same eye) are equally striking in the similarity of the properties of their two receptive fields (Hubel and Wiesel. 1971) It seems that this similarity must be determined by an innate mechanism, however difficult it may be to imagine, since.the activity from the two receptive fields would not be correlated by visual experience. If such an innate mechanism is available to the Siamese cat, then presumably it would also be available to the normal cat, eliminating the need for an experience-dependent mechanism. 111is example illustrates that teleological considerations, however seductive, can only suggest a role for early experience in the development of the visual cortex. The demonstration of such a role will require more extensive experimental evidence than we have at present. Stryker proposed that orientation selectivity, one of the organizing features of the visual cortex, develops innately in the cat as it does in the monkey. Ocular dominance, another organizing feature of the cortex, develops innately also, but it is easily modifiable in both cat and monkey by abnonnal early visual experience (Hubel et ai., 1976, 1977a; and see Wiesel, above). Present evidence suggests that orientation selectivity may not be modified by visual experience in any way except destructively, by the cells' becoming less selective.