Hatching Behavior in Turtles

Similar documents
Short-term Water Potential Fluctuations and Eggs of the Red-eared Slider Turtle (Trachemys scripta elegans)

JEZ Part A: Comparative Experimental Biology. An experimental test of the effects of fluctuating incubation temperatures on hatchling phenotype

WATER plays an important role in all stages

Rookery on the east coast of Penins. Author(s) ABDULLAH, SYED; ISMAIL, MAZLAN. Proceedings of the International Sy

CHELONIAN CONSERVATION AND BIOLOGY International Journal of Turtle and Tortoise Research

Developmental environment has long-lasting effects on behavioural performance in two turtles with environmental sex determination

Phenotypic variation in smooth softshell turtles (Apalone mutica) from eggs incubated in constant versus fluctuating temperatures

Like mother, like daughter: inheritance of nest-site

and hydration of hatchling Painted Turtles, Chrysemys picta

Thermal and fitness-related consequences of nest location in Painted Turtles (Chrysemys picta)

Weaver Dunes, Minnesota

University of Canberra. This thesis is available in print format from the University of Canberra Library.

Phenotypic Effects of Thermal Mean and Fluctuations on Embryonic Development and Hatchling Traits in a Lacertid Lizard, Takydromus septentrionalis

Postnatal effects of incubation length in mallard and pheasant chicks

Environmentally Cued Hatching in Reptiles

Reproductive physiology and eggs

The natural history of nesting in two Australian freshwater turtles.

Metabolic Heating and the Prediction of Sex Ratios for Green Turtles (Chelonia mydas)

Migration. Migration = a form of dispersal which involves movement away from and subsequent return to the same location, typically on an annual basis.

The critical importance of incubation temperature

Who Really Owns the Beach? The Competition Between Sea Turtles and the Coast Renee C. Cohen

THE adaptive significance, if any, of temperature-dependent

Diane C. Tulipani, Ph.D. CBNERRS Discovery Lab July 15, 2014 TURTLES

Pivotal range and thermosensitive period of the pig-nosed turtle, Carettochelys insculpta (Testudines: Carettochelydidae), from northern Australia

VERTEBRATE READING. Fishes

Station 1 1. (3 points) Identification: Station 2 6. (3 points) Identification:

Social and Thermal Cues Influence Nest-site Selection in a Nocturnal Gecko, Oedura lesueurii

Maternal Effects in the Green Turtle (Chelonia mydas)

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

Effects of low-oxygen conditions on embryo growth in the painted turtle, Chrysemys picta

Do TSD, sex ratios, and nest characteristics influence the vulnerability of tuatara to global warming?

The influence of propagule size and maternal nest-site. selection on survival and behaviour of neonate turtles. J. J. KOLBE* and F. J.

How Does Photostimulation Age Alter the Interaction Between Body Size and a Bonus Feeding Program During Sexual Maturation?

A comparison of placental tissue in the skinks Eulamprus tympanum and E. quoyii. Yates, Lauren A.

THE risk of predation has pervasive lethal and nonlethal

D. Burke \ Oceans First, Issue 3, 2016, pgs

Can natural phenotypic variances be estimated reliably under homogeneous laboratory conditions?

Gulf and Caribbean Research

Growth and Development. Embryonic development 2/22/2018. Timing of hatching. Hatching. Young birds and their parents

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

Consequences of Extended Egg Retention in the Eastern Fence Lizard (Sceloporus undulatus)

Does Variation in Soil Water Content Induce Variation in the Size of Hatchling Snapping Turtles (Chelydra serpentina)? MICHAEL S.

Age and Season Impact Resource Allocation to Eggs and Nesting Behavior in the Painted Turtle

phenotypes of hatchling lizards, regardless of overall mean incubation temperature

Field Herpetology Final Guide

INFLUENCE OF FEED QUALITY ON THE EXPRESSION OF POST WEANING GROWTH ASBV s IN WHITE SUFFOLK LAMBS

Amniote Relationships. Reptilian Ancestor. Reptilia. Mesosuarus freshwater dwelling reptile

B-Division Herpetology Test. By: Brooke Diamond

Final Report. Nesting green turtles of Torres Strait. Mark Hamann, Justin Smith, Shane Preston and Mariana Fuentes

EMBRYONIC TEMPERATURE INFLUENCES JUVENILE TEMPERATURE CHOICE AND GROWTH RATE IN SNAPPING TURTLES CHELYDRA SERPENTINA

Ecological Archives E A2

REPORT OF ACTIVITIES TURTLE ECOLOGY RESEARCH REPORT Crescent Lake National Wildlife Refuge 31 May to 4 July 2017

A Survey of Aquatic Turtles at Kickapoo State Park and Middle Fork State Fish and Wildlife Area (MFSFWA)

Marine Turtle Surveys on Diego Garcia. Prepared by Ms. Vanessa Pepi NAVFAC Pacific. March 2005

The Australian Freshwater Turtle Catalogue. Edition

Influence of Temperature on the Life History of Turtles:

Available from Deakin Research Online:

Is Parental Care the Key to Understanding Endothermy in Birds and Mammals?

Biology. Slide 1of 50. End Show. Copyright Pearson Prentice Hall

2 nd Term Final. Revision Sheet. Students Name: Grade: 11 A/B. Subject: Biology. Teacher Signature. Page 1 of 11

Environmental effects on fitness and consequences for sex allocation in a reptile with environmental sex determination

Phenotypic and fitness consequences of maternal nest-site choice across multiple early life stages

The significance of predation in nest site selection of turtles: an experimental consideration of macro- and microhabitat preferences

Biology Slide 1 of 50

A description of an Indo-Chinese rat snake (Ptyas korros [Schlegel, 1837]) clutch, with notes on an instance of twinning

These small issues are easily addressed by small changes in wording, and should in no way delay publication of this first- rate paper.

Return to the sea: Marine birds, reptiles and pinnipeds

Effects of nest temperature and moisture on phenotypic traits of hatchling snakes (Tropidonophis mairii, Colubridae) from tropical Australia

Natural History Note

Effects of Incubation Temperature on Growth and Performance of the Veiled Chameleon (Chamaeleo calyptratus)

Bald Head Island Conservancy 2018 Sea Turtle Report Emily Goetz, Coastal Scientist

08 alberts part2 7/23/03 9:10 AM Page 95 PART TWO. Behavior and Ecology

4 Many species of mammals, birds, reptiles, amphibians and fish 940L. Source 1 Habitats

MATERNAL NEST-SITE CHOICE AND OFFSPRING FITNESS IN A TROPICAL SNAKE (TROPIDONOPHIS MAIRII, COLUBRIDAE)

Estimating radionuclide transfer to reptiles

The ecology and sex determination of the pig-nosed turtle, Carettochelys insculpta, in the wet-dry tropics of Australia

treatments. Eggs turned until 18 d had higher pco 2 and lower po during this second phase to ensure these conditions, but

Sec KEY CONCEPT Reptiles, birds, and mammals are amniotes.

Proceedings of the 2nd Internationa. SEASTAR2000 Workshop) (2005):

Class Reptilia Testudines Squamata Crocodilia Sphenodontia

Writing: Lesson 31. Today the students will be learning how to write more advanced middle paragraphs using a variety of elaborative techniques.

Erin Maggiulli. Scientific Name (Genus species) Lepidochelys kempii. Characteristics & Traits

Climate and predation dominate juvenile and adult recruitment in a turtle with temperature-dependent sex determination

! Three things needed to survive on land were: ! 1. Have lungs and breathe air. ! 2. Have a body resistant to drying out.

Question Set 1: Animal EVOLUTIONARY BIODIVERSITY

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns

Bio4009 : Projet de recherche/research project

What s new in 2017 for TSD? Marc Girondot

Unit 19.3: Amphibians

Experimental analysis of an early life-history stage: avian predation selects for larger body size of hatchling turtles

Vertebrates. skull ribs vertebral column

d. Wrist bones. Pacific salmon life cycle. Atlantic salmon (different genus) can spawn more than once.

Statistical description of temperature-dependent sex determination using maximum likelihood

Heart rate responses to cooling in emu hatchlings

REPTILES. Scientific Classification of Reptiles To creep. Kingdom: Animalia Phylum: Chordata Subphylum: Vertebrata Class: Reptilia

A Three Year Survey of Aquatic Turtles in a Riverside Pond

Allocating Feed to Female Broiler Breeders: Technical Bulletin #2

DOES VIVIPARITY EVOLVE IN COLD CLIMATE REPTILES BECAUSE PREGNANT FEMALES MAINTAIN STABLE (NOT HIGH) BODY TEMPERATURES?

Phenotypic and fitness consequences of maternal nest-site choice across multiple early life stages

From Slime to Scales: Evolution of Reptiles. Review: Disadvantages of Being an Amphibian

Transcription:

Integrative and Comparative Biology, volume 51, number 1, pp. 100 110 doi:10.1093/icb/icr045 SYMPOSIUM Hatching Behavior in Turtles Ricky-John Spencer 1, *, and Fredric J. Janzen *Water and Wildlife Ecology Group (WWE), Native and Pest Animal Unit (NPAU), School of Natural Sciences, University of Western Sydney, Penrith South DC, Locked Bag 1797, NSW, 1797, Australia; Department of Ecology, Evolution, and Organismal Biology, Iowa State University, 253 Bessey, Ames, IA 50011, USA From the symposium Environmentally Cued Hatching across Taxa: Embryos Choose a Birthday presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Integrative and Comparative Biology, January 3 7, 2011, at Salt Lake City, Utah. 1 E-mail: ricky.spencer@uws.edu.au Synopsis Incubation temperature plays a prominent role in shaping the phenotypes and fitness of embryos, including affecting developmental rates. In many taxa, including turtles, eggs are deposited in layers such that thermal gradients alter developmental rates within a nest. Despite this thermal effect, a nascent body of experimental work on environmentally cued hatching in turtles has revealed unexpected synchronicity in hatching behavior. This review discusses environmental cues for hatching, physiological mechanisms behind synchronous hatching, proximate and ultimate causes for this behavior, and future directions for research. Four freshwater turtle species have been investigated experimentally, with hatching in each species elicited by different environmental cues and responding via various physiological mechanisms. Hatching of groups of eggs in turtles apparently involves some level of embryo embryo communication and thus is not a purely passive activity. Although turtles are not icons of complex social behavior, life-history theory predicts that the group environment of the nest can drive the evolution of environmentally cued hatching. Introduction Developing embryos are exposed to numerous environmental factors that elicit a diversity of phenotypic and fitness effects that are manifested both before and after birth (e.g., Deeming and Ferguson 1991). In oviparous species, perhaps the most influential environmental factor during embryonic development is temperature. Within the nonlethal range, for example, higher incubation temperatures notably accelerate embryonic development and thereby reduce time until hatching from eggs (e.g., Andrews 2004). This timing of birth has enormous implications for both immediate and future survival. In animals that produce more than one offspring, assessing environmental cues is important to synchronize birth with siblings, thus it is not surprising that synchrony in the timing of births has evolved in many species (O Donoghue and Boutin 1995). However, many factors promote intra clutch variation in incubation period in oviparous species, including differences in egg size, order of ovulation, and disparate thermal microenvironments of eggs (Andrews 2004). Despite these sources of developmental asynchrony, synchronous hatching through alteration of incubation period occurs in many oviparous taxa, including invertebrates (Frechette and Coderre 2000), fishes (Bradbury et al. 2005), amphibians (Sih and Moore 1993; Warkentin 1995, 2000), crocodilians (Ferguson 1985), squamates (Vitt 1991), turtles (Doody et al. 2001; Spencer et al. 2001), and birds (Lack 1968; Vince 1969; Davies and Cooke 1983). Data from studies of reptiles do not feature prominently in our understanding of environmentally cued hatching. However, turtles comprise one group where elements of this concept have been the subject of experimentation (Doody et al. 2001; Spencer et al. 2001; Colbert et al. 2010). Turtles ovulate clutches of eggs simultaneously, and embryonic development is arrested within the oviducts at the gastrula stage until oviposition (Ewert 1985). However, as the eggs are generally deposited in several layers within a nest, environmental gradients Advanced Access publication June 9, 2011 ß The Author 2011. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Society for Integrative and Comparative Biology. All rights reserved. For permissions please email: journals.permissions@oup.com.

Hatching behavior in turtles 101 alter developmental rates among clutchmates (Maloney et al. 1990; Gyuris 1993; Thompson 1997). In particular, eggs near the top of a nest experience higher temperatures (up to 68C higher in nests of the Murray River turtle, Emydura macquarii), which increases developmental rates and shortens incubation relative to eggs near the bottom of the chamber (Thompson 1988, 1989; Booth and Thompson 1991). Thus, hatching synchrony presumably should not occur within a turtle nest because incubation times should differ between eggs at the top and bottom of the nest. Arguably, the best-known example of environmentally cued hatching is the image of hatchling sea turtles emerging from the nest en masse. However, this scenario confounds hatching from eggs with emergence from the nest and does not clarify mechanism in either case. For example, embryos might hatch synchronously because temperatures are relatively similar between the top and bottom of the nest (Fig. 1) or because they are responding to other cues. Similarly, hatchlings dig upwards from the nest chamber (Bustard 1967), but if they approach the surface during daylight, hatchlings will wait until dusk to emerge from the nest (Bustard 1967; Mrosovsky 1968, 1980). Mass emergence from the nest by sea turtles thus may result from delayed emergence rather than environmentally cued synchronous hatching. Hence, freshwater turtles have been used as nascent models to experimentally assess hatching behavior because variability of temperatures within their nests (Fig. 1) conveniently disconnects temperature, embryonic development, and the timing of hatching. Incubation temperature is the primary determinant of incubation times in reptiles (Dmi el 1967; Sexton and Marion 1974; Mrosovsky and Yntema 1980; Miller 1985; Gutzke and Packard 1987; Packard et al. 1987), but its influence on hatching from eggs per se is not known. The timing of hatching may be phenotypically plastic within certain boundaries set by incubation temperature, but the actual cues for hatching derive from a range of biotic and abiotic factors. Factors such as predation, hypoxia, dehydration, and flooding occur commonly in a range of ectothermic vertebrates (fish: Martin and Swiderski 2001; Speer-Blank and Martin 2004; amphibians: Warkentin 1995, 2000; reptiles: Doody et al. 2001; Spencer et al. 2001). With many of these taxa, hatching is spontaneous because embryonic development throughout a clutch is complete and the species can enter embryonic aestivation until conditions for hatching are optimal (Georges et al. 2008). Less common in nature is early hatching. Red-eyed treefrogs, Agalychnis callidryas, hatch early in response to egg-eating snakes and wasps (Warkentin 1995, 2000), as do the frogs Hyla regilla and Rana cascadae in response to egg-eating leeches (Chivers et al. 2001). The frog Hyperolius cinnamomeoventris hatches at a smaller size when exposed to egg-eating fly larvae (Vonesh 2000). Synchronous or spontaneous hatching is less likely to occur when eggs are developing at different rates and hatching cues occur well before the entire clutch has completed development. In such cases, there are likely consequences for both short- and long-term fitness, which provide opportunities to test ecological and evolutionary trade-off theories of optimality in the emerging field of embryonic behavior. This review focuses on the behavior of synchronous hatching in turtles and incorporates unpublished findings because of the nascence of this research area. We discuss the environmental cues for hatching, the physiological mechanisms behind synchronous hatching, the proximate and ultimate causes for this behavior in the context of optimality theory, and future directions for research. Defining synchronous hatching Synchronous emergence of neonatal turtles from the nest is well documented (De Pari 1996; Tucker 1997, 1999; Nagle et al. 2004), although it is not ubiquitous (e.g., Kolbe and Janzen 2002), with many marine turtles hatching throughout the day but delaying emergence until dusk or night (Bustard 1967; Mrosovsky 1968, 1980). Indeed, social facilitation behavior of hatchling green turtles (Chelonia mydas) occurs during emergence from the nest (Carr and Hirth 1961). But such emergence behavior should not be confused with synchronous hatching, which refers to coordinated departure from eggs of fully formed embryos (i.e., hatchlings). Experimental evidence of environmentally cued hatching in turtles has focused on three freshwater species, where thermal variability in the shallow nests is well established (Thompson 1988). Pig-nose turtles (Carettochelys insculpta) can hatch spontaneously, embodying the traditional view of synchronous hatching as described above for various anurans. Two other species of freshwater turtle have displayed some capabilities to hatch synchronously by responding to developmental and hatching cues of clutch mates: E. macquarii can hatch synchronously under warmer incubation temperature regimes (Spencer et al. 2001), and the painted turtle, Chrysemys picta, was unable to hatch synchronously in experimental protocols, despite clearly adjusting hatching

102 R.-J. Spencer and F. J. Janzen Fig. 1 Temperature at various depths in freshwater turtle nesting areas at Albury, Australia. Data loggers (IBUTTON TM ) were placed at 5 depths (5 60 cm), and the hole was refilled with the original sand (predominantly river sand) with little or no vegetation. Ten plots were randomly located throughout the nesting beach (see Spencer 2002; Spencer et al. 2006) for 7 days (January 2 9, 2000). Average (SD) temperatures are shown at each depth recorded. Small medium-sized freshwater turtles construct nests 5 20 cm deep (light gray region), and sea turtles construct nests 440 cm deep (dark gray region). times (Colbert et al. 2010). Spontaneous or synchronous hatching in species that hatch early or accelerate development (as opposed to delaying hatching) is difficult to achieve, thus for the remainder of this review the term synchronous hatching should be understood to include any response of an embryo to the developmental stage or hatching behavior of clutch mates, which may not necessarily result in spontaneous hatching, but does result in the adjustment of incubation times. Such synchronous hatching occurs in precocial birds (Vince and Chinn 1971) and turtles (Spencer et al. 2001, Colbert et al. 2010). Synchronous hatching and turtles There has long been suspicion that hatching in sea turtles is somewhat coordinated, similar to their emergence from the nest. However, given that these large animals construct deep nests where both the thermal gradient and variance between the top and bottom of the nest is low (Packard and Packard 1988), there is no conclusive evidence that embryos respond to anything other than temperature. Much research into sea turtle biology has focused on cues for emergence (Bustard 1967; Mrosovsky 1968, 1980; Gyuris 1993) and hatchling orientation postnest emergence (Mrosovsky 1978; Salmon and Lohmann 1989; Lohmann 1991), thus, these turtles now need experimental study of hatching synchrony. As mentioned earlier, three freshwater turtle species have a demonstrated capacity to hatch synchronously in the broad sense of the term (Spencer et al. 2001; Georges et al. 2008; Colbert et al. 2010). One other species (Chelodina longicollis) has conclusively demonstrated that it does not have the capacity to either delay or speed up hatching to ensure synchrony (R.-J. Spencer, unpublished data). Pig-nose turtle nesting and embryonic developmental behavior is primarily geared to predictable weather patterns of northern Australia (Georges et al. 2008). The response of C. insculpta embryos to rising water levels promotes synchronous hatching, but there is some evidence that biotic factors like vibration (see below) are also cues for hatching (Georges et al. 2008). Full-developed offspring only hatch from eggs when a nest is inundated with river water. There is also no evidence that pig-nose turtles hatch in a premature developmental state. On the other hand, the Australian turtle E. macquarii (Spencer et al. 2001) and the North American turtle C. picta (Colbert et al. 2010) adjust incubation period in response to some unknown factor(s) related to developmental stage of sibling embryos to ensure that hatching of eggs within a clutch occurs over a short time period. In contrast, the

Hatching behavior in turtles 103 Australian turtle (C. longicollis) does not hatch synchronously or adjust incubation times independent of temperature (R.-J. Spencer, unpublished data). Mechanisms and cues of synchronous hatching in turtles Pig-nose turtles nest late in the dry season from mid-july to early November in northern Australia (Georges and Kennett 1988, Doody et al. 2003). The incubation and hatching biology of C. insculpta is different in comparison to most turtles. Eggs require 64 74 days at 308C to develop to a point where hatching is possible (i.e., until yolk internalization) after which they enter embryonic aestivation (Webb et al. 1986). At the onset of aestivation, metabolic rate decreases, embryonic growth ceases, and yolk is used during diapause at a rate that yields an extra 59 days until yolk is exhausted (at 28 308C; Webb et al. 1986). The proximal cue for hatching is anoxia, which can be caused by immersing eggs in water or in a nitrogen-only atmosphere (Webb et al. 1986). In the field, torrential rain or flooding stimulates hatching (Georges 1992), an adaptation that synchronizes hatching with the onset of the wet season. Although a mature hatchling can remain in the egg for 59 days to yolk exhaustion, the anoxia stimulus to hatch in the field normally occurs within 20 days (Doody et al. 2001) after the eggs are developmentally mature (Georges et al. 2004, 2005, 2008). Hatching of C. insculpta is typically explosive (Webb et al. 1986; Doody et al. 2001): On immersion of eggs, the turtles hatch within minutes. Behavior of hatching clutch mates may also be important. In laboratory experiments, groups of eggs hatched faster than single eggs treated in the same way, indicating a function of sibling vibrations (Georges et al. 2008). The ability of C. insculpta to hatch early prior to the aestivation period is limited. Nest mortality due to flooding was 20% when turtles nested late after a below average wet season (Doody et al. 2004). Most nests destroyed by floods were constructed at low elevations and had not completed development by the time flooding began (Doody et al. 2004). In contrast, C. picta and E. macquarii do not display developmental aestivation during incubation and have the capability of shortening the incubation period to hatch synchronously or early (Spencer et al. 2001; Colbert et al. 2010). Although the nesting cues may differ slightly between the species, the timing of nesting and incubation period is similar between C. picta and E. macquarii (Bowen et al. 2005). Nesting generally occurs in late spring-early summer in both species and nests are usually constructed close to water (Bowen et al. 2005). Clutch sizes vary depending on female size and location, but both species construct nests that are about 5 15 cm deep (Jackson et al. 2004). Incubation lasts 72 80 days in C. picta nests (Zweifel 1989) and 66 85 days in E. macquarii nests (Cann 1998). The major difference between the natural history of nests of C. picta and E. macquarii is that painted turtle hatchlings in the northern part of their range typically arrange themselves symmetrically in the nest and overwinter to emerge the following spring (Costanzo et al. 2008), whereas no delayed emergence occurs in E. macquarii (Cann 1998). Both these freshwater turtle species do not delay hatching, unlike C. insculpta, and they can hatch prematurely (Colbert et al. 2010) or compensate by increasing developmental rates (J. McGlashan et al., submitted for publication) to synchronize hatching. Respiration rates in reptiles and precocial birds generally drop by up to 25% before hatching occurs (Birchard and Reiber 1995; Birchard 2000; Peterson and Kruegl 2005), but hatching can occur at any time after peak metabolism. The fall in metabolism prior to hatching is present in some species as resting, or the secondary developmental stage, which is variable in length (Fig. 2). If turtles like C. picta do not adjust their developmental rate, they may significantly shorten the secondary development stage (Fig. 2), which could reduce post-hatching fitness (Colbert et al. 2010). Premature hatching in C. picta was inferred from behavioral and morphological assessments of neonates (Colbert et al. 2010). That is, turtles that were known or suspected to have hatched prematurely exhibited reduced ability to turn themselves when overturned in performance trials and typically had substantial residual yolk extruding from the abdominal cavity. In contrast, a recent study monitored embryonic rates of development in E. macquarii throughout the entire incubation period and found that these turtles do not hatch in a premature developmental state because less developed embryos responded to the presence of more advanced eggs within a clutch by increasing metabolic and heart rates. The assumption is that turtles have limited capacity to regulate metabolic processes independent of temperature, because they are ectothermic and thermoconformers. Hence, increases in metabolic rates and embryonic development above temperature regimes are improbable and early, or synchronous hatching, should be achieved through incomplete development (Colbert et al. 2010). Both _VCO 2 and heart rate of E. macquarii increased in response to

104 R.-J. Spencer and F. J. Janzen 1.2 Secondary Development 1 Vo 2 (ml.h -1 ) 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 Group Incubation Begins 0 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 Incubation time (days) Fig. 2 Metabolic profile of turtle embryos (eggs) throughout the incubation period. Solid black line represents the stimulus eggs that have been incubated separately at warmer temperatures for the first week of development. The gray line represents eggs that are not stimulated to increase their developmental rates throughout incubation, but reduce their secondary developmental stage to hatch synchronously with the stimulus eggs. The dashed line represents eggs that are stimulated to increase their developmental rates and hatch at a similar developmental stage as the stimulus eggs. The gray regions represent the secondary developmental stage. The secondary developmental stage of eggs that simply hatch early is approximately half (dark gray region) of that of the other eggs (light gray þ dark gray regions). Based on the metabolic profile of E. macquarii (Thompson 1989). From J. McGlashan et al. (unpublished data). the developmental stage of neighboring eggs. This outcome supports the dashed-line scenario illustrated in the graphical model of the catchup hypothesis for synchronous hatching (Fig. 2). However, differences in _VCO 2 and heart rate in stimulated E. macquarii embryos do not manifest until the last one-third of incubation, rather than throughout the incubation period as illustrated in Fig. 2. How development is accelerated is unknown. Turtle embryonic development requires a fixed number of heart beats (Du et al. 2009); thus, faster heart rates induced by warmer temperatures should result in shorter incubation times. But heart rate is not fixed even at constant temperatures, hence incubation temperature may determine the upper and lower ranges, rather than a specific heart rate, for an individual. Heart rate in E. macquarii can vary by up to 15% around the average rate throughout a day, with diurnal peaks and troughs throughout the entire incubation period (F. K. Loudon et al., unpublished data) (Fig. 3). It may be possible for embryonic turtles in the presence of more advanced eggs to maintain increased heart rates and thus develop faster than normal rates. However, diurnal patterns of heart rate have not been compared between more- and less-developed embryos within a clutch. At a metabolic level, maturation of cardiovascular control mechanisms in late-term embryos may also be important. Slight physiological increases in metabolic (and thus, developmental) rate could provide a means to accelerate embryonic development and growth, given the inability of an embryo to thermoregulate behaviorally. However, mechanisms responsible for ontogenetic shifts in heart rates and metabolism are unknown, but may relate to maturation of both hormonal and nervous regulatory systems (Crossley et al. 2003). One possible factor is thyroid hormone, which is crucial in growth, development, and function of most vertebrate tissues, such as brain, bone, fat, and muscle (Brent 2000). Thyroid hormone affects embryonic tissue accretion and differentiation through both metabolic and nonmetabolic mechanisms (Fowden 1995). Critically, thyroid hormone affects various metabolic pathways, especially energy metabolism and also plays a role in mammalian and reptilian metabolic responses to temperature acclimation (Himms- Hagen 1983; Stamper et al. 1990; O Steen and Janzen 1999; Du et al. 2010). Although thyroid hormone is a key regulator of postnatal growth and the complex processes of transition from allantoic to pulmonary respiration (Decuypere et al. 1991; Dewil et al. 1996; Cassar-Malek et al. 2007), the thyroid is also one of the earliest endocrine organs to differentiate and (Lynn 1960) thus the metabolic adjustment of turtle embryos in these experiments could lie with changes in thyroid hormones. Indeed, thyroid hormone plays a role in length of

Hatching behavior in turtles 105 Fig. 3 Diurnal shifts from mean heart rate of eggs incubated at 268C and 308C at various stages before and after peak heart rates. Data for up to a month before hatching were pooled, and the x axis represents hours away from the peak heart rate (from F. K. Loudon et al., unpublished data). the incubation process of chickens and hypoxic conditions induce its production in embryos (Decuypere et al. 1991; Dewil et al. 1996; Buys et al. 1998; De Smit et al. 2006). The cues for hatching in C. picta and E. macquarii are unknown and may differ between the species, as do the mechanisms for synchronous hatching. With C. picta hatching at a developmentally premature stage, the cues for hatching are likely to be localized toward the end of incubation when more advanced embryos begin to hatch. Avian and crocodilian embryos may communicate developmental stage by means of audible clicks (Driver 1965; Ferguson 1985). Additional cues might come from vibrations of eggs in close contact at hatching or other means of physical disturbance, such as a predator opening a nest or disturbing a clutch (Vitt 1991; Warkentin 1995, 2000). However, vocalization is not known in embryonic or hatchling turtles, and there is no contact between eggs during incubation in these synchrony experiments (Spencer et al. 2001; Colbert et al. 2010; J. McGlashan et al., unpublished data; R.-J. Spencer, unpublished data). Spencer et al. (2001) note that sounds produced by pipping eggs could be a potential cue, as well as the changes in oxygen consumption, CO 2 production, and heart rate that are characteristic of the overall decrease in metabolic rate before hatching (Birchard and Reiber 1995; Birchard 2000; Peterson and Kruegl 2005). As described above, E. macquarii embryos are stimulated to increase developmental rates well before hatching. The increase in metabolic rates during the last third of development in E. macquarii, indicates that both elevated CO 2 levels and heart rates are plausible explanations for a potential hatching cue (Spencer et al. 2001). With eggs not incubated in contact with each other, differences in heart rate may only be detected during the last phase of incubation when the heartbeat strengthens. Similarly, CO 2 levels in a nest may only reach high concentrations during the last phases of incubation when embryonic development and metabolism accelerate. High CO 2 levels within a nest may indicate imminent hatching and may cue the production of thyroid hormone, as in chickens (Decuypere et al. 1991), and increased development of less advanced embryos. Audible cues from pipping eggs and gas exchange within a nest thus may be responsible for synchronous or early hatching in E. macquarii and other species. The close proximity of eggs and likely higher CO 2 concentrations in nests relative to the lab experiment accentuate the ecological probability of this scenario. Proximate and ultimate significance of synchronous hatching in turtles Life-history theory predicts that the timing of transitions between different life stages should vary with the costs and benefits accruing in current and succeeding stages (Werner and Gilliam 1984). Adaptive responses of posthatching animals to predators are

106 R.-J. Spencer and F. J. Janzen common and include morphological changes, life-history shifts, and behavioral responses (Lima and Dill 1989; Harvell 1990). With such a diverse range of mechanisms underpinning synchronous hatching in turtles, the ecological, physiological, and evolutionary causes are also likely to be diverse. Collectively, embryonic aestivation, explosive hatching, and sibling communication in Carettochelys are unique adaptations for matching timing of hatching with the onset of the wet season, especially important, given the high annual variation in timing of nesting (Doody et al. 2001, 2004). The proximal cue for hatching is anoxia and, as water levels increase, clarity of the aquatic environment declines and predation risk of hatchlings may further decrease. Timing emergence from the nest with the onset of the wet season may also maximize food availability for hatchlings (Webb et al. 1986; Doody et al. 2001). In Papua New Guinea, though, hatchlings may have to contend with osmoregulatory challenges because many nests are constructed on coastal beaches where salinity of the surrounding water can approach ocean water levels (Georges et al. 2008). Irrespective of any synchronized hatching, hatchling C. insculpta emerge from the nest as nest temperatures are decreasing, at night (Doody et al. 2001). The timing of hatching, the transition from egg to larval and juvenile stages, varies with risk of mortality to both embryos and hatchlings (Sih and Moore 1993, Warkentin 1995, 2000). The link between synchronous hatching and group emergence has been tied closely to the concept of predator swamping. Predator swamping is typically invoked, even if only implicitly, as an explanation for temporal and spatial synchrony in behavior among individuals in a population (Tucker et al. 2008). Among these instances where per capita predation is presumably diluted by synchrony are included the iconic arribada nesting of sea turtles (Pritchard 1969) and behavioral synchrony of hatchling turtles in nests (Tucker et al. 2008). In the olive ridley sea turtle (Lepidochelys olivacea), first-night predation was higher for solitary nests compared with arribada nests (Eckrich and Owens 1995). Tucker (1997) suggested that synchronous nesting in the red-eared slider turtle (Trachemys scripta elegans) was a predator-swamping tactic (Tucker 1997; Tucker et al. 2008). Most predators on hatchling turtles are generalists (e.g., Janzen et al. 2000), so the factors responsible for synchronization of hatching or emergence may involve prey switching rather than predator swamping (Ims 1990). Indeed, Tucker et al. (2008) released T. s. elegans neonates in different-sized groups and found no support for the predator-swamping hypothesis. Instead, neonates emerging from nests, and particularly those emerging early, had the highest survivorship. Prey that are first to perform a particular behavior may have better chances for survival due to the lag time before predators switch to new prey (Ims 1990; Testa 2002). The prey-switching hypothesis also supports selection for the catchup mechanisms of synchronous hatching in C. picta, E. macquarii, and, possibly, C. insculpta. This developmental pattern makes sense if selection favors early synchronous emergence from nests (in accordance with prey switching) rather than delayed synchronous emergence (in accordance with predator swamping) (Tucker et al. 2008). Substantial costs are associated with the catchup or early-hatching mechanisms for synchronous hatching. Increases in metabolic rate by E. macquarii translate directly into increases in rate of embryonic development and utilization of yolk reserves, which are vital for the initial few weeks of neonatal development and survival (J. McGlashan et al., unpublished data). However, the costs of hatching prematurely are likely to be more immediate. The trade-off between the benefits of premature hatching and the costs are primarily embedded in the secondary development period of incubation (Fig. 2). Varying the length of this stage may have both short- and long-term costs because secondary development is associated with the maturation of the neuromuscular system, whereas primary development (up to the peak metabolic rate) is associated with organ and tissue development. Examples of such costs are reflected by Japanese quail chicks (Coturnix coturnix japonica) with accelerated development, which take 1 2 h later to stand than normal chicks (Vince and Chinn 1971), and by early-hatching C. picta, whose neuromuscular function is reduced for at least 9 months after hatching (Colbert et al. 2010). However, a subsequent release experiment involving these C. picta neonates found that these performance disadvantages did not appear to translate into reduced survival early in life (P. L. Colbert et al., unpublished data). Selection for early hatching without reduced capabilities may be far stronger in E. macquarii compared with C. picta because the Australian turtle emerges from the nest much sooner than the North American turtle. Indeed, hatchlings in some populations of C. picta can spend more than 9 months overwintering within a nest (Costanzo et al. 2008) and selection may favor individuals that minimize the mobilization of yolk reserves during incubation because they are required to survive overwintering. Position within the nest

Hatching behavior in turtles 107 also has profound implications for winter survival in C. picta (Costanzo et al. 2008), thus, individuals hatching first may have a distinct advantage in securing optimal overwintering sites within the nest (i.e., surrounded by clutch mates near the bottom of the nest). Winter conditions within nest chambers can present a serious challenge to survival, even for a cold-adapted species such as the painted turtle (Costanzo et al. 2008). Nest thermal gradients that differentially affect developmental rates of clutch mates in the summer may also have differential effects on freezing mortality in the winter (Weisrock and Janzen 1999). Hence, despite both species hatching earlier than expected, the ultimate and proximate (i.e., increased developmental rates vs. premature hatching) mechanisms for synchronous hatching in C. picta may be very different from those in E. macquarii. In contrast to C. picta, growth and performance of accelerated E. macquarii is not significantly different from normally incubated embryos, at hatching and even several months after hatching (J. McGlashan et al., unpublished data). Even in species where synchronous or early hatching occurs, disparate proximate mechanisms probably underlie superficially similar phenotypic traits (Colbert et al. 2010; J. McGlashan et al., unpublished data). Colbert et al. (2010) suggested that phylogenetic inertia may be responsible for its presence in modern day turtles, but for a trait to remain common in many species, it must, at a minimum, offer no selective disadvantage to individuals displaying it. Clearly, less-developed embryos in C. picta are potentially disadvantaged, in terms of neuromuscular capabilities, by hatching early. Also in species that synchronize nesting and nest in high densities, the dilution or predator swamping effect in reducing the individual risk of predation may be a strong positive selective force, but with solitary nesters, the disadvantage of synchronous hatching (reduced coordination and resources) may far exceed any advantages of group emergence from a nest. Moreover, the trait must be conserved across taxa. In three species of freshwater turtles where hatching has been experimentally determined to be synchronous (or partially synchronous), the mechanisms by which it occurs are very different. A fourth species, C. longicollis, does not hatch synchronously or even show signs of developmental adjustment in response to moreor less-advanced siblings within a clutch (R.-J. Spencer, submitted for publication). Thus, the behavior of hatching in turtles in response to environmental cues currently has enigmatic evolutionary implications. Conclusions and future research This review highlights that hatching behavior in turtles is poorly understood. However, although temperature is the major factor controlling embryonic development in ectotherms, its effects on hatching times may be less substantial. The plasticity of hatching times in turtles may have upper and lower boundaries roughly set by developmental stage, within which specific biotic and abiotic factors serve as key cues for actual hatching. These cues, particularly in turtles that are hatching early, are poorly known and require further in-depth experimentation. For example, communication between siblings within nests is not a new phenomenon (e.g., chicks [Driver 1965] and crocodiles [Ferguson 1985]). However, less obvious cues drive synchronous hatching in some species, so much so that they induce accelerated developmental rates at certain stages of the incubation period (J. McGlashan et al., submitted for publication). The physiological mechanisms behind this plasticity of developmental rate are not known but could even have implications for understanding the evolution of endothermy in vertebrates. This review identifies four different mechanisms of hatching behavior in turtles from the only four species that have been assessed experimentally (Spencer et al. 2001; Georges et al. 2008; Colbert et al. 2010; R.-J. Spencer, unpublished data). The breadth of behaviors involved in development and hatching of embryonic turtles is only just being revealed, and clearly more experimentation is required in species that have shown some propensity to exhibit environmentally cued hatching. Similarly, the proximate and ultimate causes for these behaviors are not known and require both physiological experimentation on embryonic development and hatchling release experiments to assess longer-term implications for fitness. Hatching in turtles is not a passive activity and there is clear, but currently limited, evidence for embryo embryo communication in turtles. Reptiles are not icons of complex social behavior, but in the group environment of the nest, optimality or trade-off theory predicts that behavior, such as environmentally cued hatching, can evolve. Acknowledgments We are particularly grateful to K. Martin, K. Warkentin, and R. Strauthman for the invitation to present a manuscript to ICB and for organizing the symposium Environmentally Cued Hatching across Taxa: Embryos Choose A Birthday at the SICB 2011 annual conference. We also thank the

108 R.-J. Spencer and F. J. Janzen Division of Animal Behavior (SICB) for sponsoring the symposium. This review was stimulated by valuable discussions with J. McGlashan and M. Thompson. Funding A research grant from the School of Natural Sciences at the University of Western Sydney supported travel to the symposium. References Andrews RM. 2004. Patterns of embryonic development. In: Deeming DC, editor. Reptilian incubation; environment, evolution, and behaviour. Nottingham, UK: Nottingham University Press. p. 75 102. Birchard GF. 2000. An ontogenetic shift in the response of heart rates to temperature in the developing snapping turtle (Chelydra serpentina). J Therm Biol 25:287 91. Birchard GF, Reiber CL. 1995. Effect of temperature on growth, metabolism, and chorioallantoic vascular density of developing snapping turtles (Chelydra serpentina). Physiol Zool 65:799 811. Booth DT, Thompson MB. 1991. A comparison of reptilian eggs with those of megapode birds. In: Deeming DC, Ferguson MWJ, editors. Egg incubation: Its effects on embryonic development in birds and reptiles. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 325 44. Bowen KD, Spencer R-J, Janzen FJ. 2005. A comparative study of environmental factors that affect nesting in Australian and North American freshwater turtles. J Zool 267:397 404. Bradbury IR, Campana SE, Bentzen P, Snelgrove PVR. 2005. Synchronized hatch and its ecological significance in rainbow smelt Osmerus mordax in St. Mary s Bay, Newfoundland. Limn Oceanog 86:1873 82. Brent GA. 2000. Tissue-specific actions of thyroid hormone: Insights from animal models. Rev Endocr Metab Disord 1:27 33. Bustard HR. 1967. Mechanism of nocturnal emergence from the nest in green turtle hatchlings. Nature 214:317. Buys N, Dewil E, Gonzales E, Decuypere E. 1998. Different CO 2 levels during incubation interact with hatching time and ascites susceptibility in two broiler lines selected for different growth rate. Av Pathol 27:605 12. Cann J. 1998. Australian freshwater turtles. Singapore: Beaumont Publishing Pte Ltd. Carr A, Hirth H. 1961. Social facilitation in green turtle siblings. Anim Behav 9:68 70. Cassar-Malek I, Picard B, Kah S, Hocquette JF. 2007. Relationships between thyroid status, tissue oxidative metabolism, and muscle differentiation in bovine foetuses. Dom An Endo 33:91 106. Chivers DP, Kiesecker JM, Marco A, DeVito J, Anderson MT, Blaustein AR. 2001. Predator-induced life-history changes in amphibians: egg predation induces hatching. Oikos 92:135 42. Colbert PL, Spencer R-J, Janzen FJ. 2010. Mechanism and cost of synchronous hatching. Funct Ecol 24:112 21. Costanzo JP, Lee RE Jr, Ultsch GR. 2008. Physiological ecology of overwintering in hatchling turtles. J Exp Zool Part A 309:297 37. Crossley DA, Bagatto BR, Dzialowski EM, Burggren WW. 2003. Maturation of cardiovascular control mechanisms in the embryonic emu (Dromiceius novaehollandiae). J Exp Biol 206:2703 10. Davies JC, Cooke F. 1983. Intraclutch hatch synchronization in the lesser snow goose. Can J Zool 59:1201 4. Decuypere E, Dewil E, Kühn ER. 1991. The hatching process and the role of hormones. In: Tallet SG, editor. Avian incubation. 2nd ed. London: Butterworth-Heinemann. p. 239 56. Deeming DC, Ferguson MWJ. 1991. Egg incubation: Its effects on embryonic development in birds and reptiles. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. De Pari JA. 1996. Overwintering in the nest chamber by hatchling painted turtles, Chrysemys picta, in northern New Jersey. Chelonian Conserv Biol 2:5 12. De Smit L, Bruggeman V, Tona JK, Debonne M, Onagbesan O, Arckens L, De Baerdmaeker J, Decuypere E. 2006. Embryonic developmental plasticity of the chick: increased CO 2 during the early stages of incubation changes the developmental trajectories during prenatal and postnatal growth. Comp Biochem Physiol 145A:166 75. Dewil E, Buys N, Albers GAA, Decuypere E. 1996. Different characteristics in chick embryos of two broiler lines differing in susceptibility to ascites. Br Poult Sci 37:1003 13. Dmi el R. 1967. Studies on reproduction, growth, and feeding in the snake Spalerosophis cliffordi (Colubridae). Copeia 1967:332 46. Doody JS, Georges A, Young JE. 2003. Twice every second year: reproduction in the pig-nosed turtle, Carettochelys insculpta, in the wet-dry tropics of Australia. J Zool 259:179 88. Doody JS, Georges A, Young JE. 2004. Determinants of reproductive success and offspring sex in a turtle with environmental sex determination. Biol J Linn Soc 80:1 16. Doody JS, Georges A, Young JE, Pauza M, Pepper AL, Alderman RL, Welsh MA. 2001. Embryonic aestivation and emergence behaviour in the pig-nosed turtle, Carettochelys insculpta. Can J Zool 79:1062 72. Driver PM. 1965. Clicking in the egg-young of nidifugous birds. Nature 206:315. Du WG, Radder RS, Sun B, Shine R. 2009. Determinants of incubation period: do reptilian embryos hatch after a fixed total number of heart beats? J Exp Biol 212:1302 6. Du WG, Zhao B, Shine R. 2010. Embryos in the fast lane: high-temperature heart rates of turtles decline after hatching. PLoS ONE 5:e9557.

Hatching behavior in turtles 109 Eckrich CE, Owens DW. 1995. Solitary versus arribada nesting in the olive ridley sea turtles (Lepidochelys olivacea): a test of the predator-satiation hypothesis. Herpetologica 51:349 54. Ewert MA. 1985. Embryology of turtles. In: Gans C, Billet F, Maderson PFA, editors. Biology of the Reptilia, Vol. 14. New York: John Wiley and Sons. p. 76 267. Ferguson MWJ. 1985. Reproductive biology and embryology of the crocodilians. In: Gans C, Billet F, Maderson PFA, editors. Biology of the Reptilia, Vol. 14. New York: John Wiley & Sons. p. 329 491. Fowden AL. 1995. Endocrine regulation of fetal growth. Reprod Fertil Dev 7:351 63. Frechette B, Coderre D. 2000. Oviposition strategy of the green lacewing Chrysoperla rufilabris (Neuroptera: Chrysopidae) in response to extraguild prey availability. Euro J Entomol 76:177 85. Georges A. 1992. Thermal characteristics and sex determination in field nests of the pig-nosed turtle, Carettochelys insculpta (Chelonia: Carettochelydidae), from northern Australia. Aust J Zool 40:511 21. Georges A, Begg SK, Young JE, Doody JS. 2005. Modelling reptilian development under fluctuating temperature regimes. Physiol Biochem Zool 78:18 30. Georges A, Doody JS, Begg SK, Young J. 2004. Thermal models of TSD under laboratory and field conditions. In: Valenzuela N, Lance V, editors. Temperature dependent sex determination in reptiles. Washington: Smithsonian Institution Press. p. 79 89. Georges A, Doody JS, Eisemberg C, Alacs EA, Rose M. 2008. Carettochelys insculpta Ramsay 1886 pig-nosed turtle, Fly River turtle. In: Rhodin AGJ, Pritchard PCH, van Dijk PP, Saumure RA, Buhlmann KA, Iverson JB, editors. Conservation biology of freshwater turtles and tortoises: a compilation project of the IUCN/SSC tortoise and freshwater turtle specialist group. Chelonian Research Monographs No. 5. p. 009.1.17. Georges A, Kennett R. 1988. Dry-season distribution and ecology of the Warradjan (Carettochelys insculpta Ramsay) in Kakadu National Park. Canberra: Water Research Centre. p. 1 61. Gutzke WHN, Packard GC. 1987. Influence of the hydric and thermal environments on eggs and hatchlings of bull snakes Pituophis melanoleucus. Physiol Zool 60:9 17. Gyuris E. 1993. Factors that control the emergence of green turtle hatchlings from the nest. Wildl Res 20:345 53. Harvell CD. 1990. The ecology and evolution of inducible defenses. Quart Rev Biol 65:323 39. Himms-Hagen J. 1983. Brown adipose tissue thermogenesis in obese animals. Nutr Rev 41:261 7. Ims RA. 1990. On the adaptive value of reproductive synchrony as a predator-swamping strategy. Am Nat 136:485 98. Jackson DC, Rauer EM, Feldman RA, Reese SA. 2004. Avenues of extrapulmonary oxygen uptake in western painted turtles (Chrysemys picta belli) at 108C. Comp Biochem Physiol 139A:221 7. Janzen FJ, Tucker JK, Paukstis GL. 2000. Experimental analysis of an early life-history stage: selection on size of hatchling turtles. Ecology 81:2290 304. Kolbe JJ, Janzen FJ. 2002. Experimental analysis of an early life-history stage: water loss and migrating hatchling turtles. Copeia 2002:220 6. Lack D. 1968. Ecological adaptations for breeding in birds. London: Methuen and Co. Ltd. Lima SL, Dill LM. 1989. Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68:619 40. Lohmann KJ. 1991. Magnetic orientation by hatchling loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta). J Exp Biol 155:37 49. Lynn WG. 1960. Structure and functions of the thyroid gland in reptiles. Am Midl Nat 64:309 26. Maloney JE, Darian-Smith C, Takahashi Y, Limpus CJ. 1990. The environment for development of the embryonic loggerhead turtle (Caretta caretta) in Queensland (Australia). Copeia 1990:378 87. Martin KLM, Swiderski DL. 2001. Beach spawning in fishes: phylogenetic tests of hypotheses. Am Zool 41:526 37. Miller JD. 1985. Embryology of marine turtles. In: Gans C, Billet F, Maderson PFA, editors. Biology of the Reptilia, Vol. 14. New York: John Wiley and Sons. p. 269 328. Mrosovsky N. 1968. Nocturnal emergence of hatchling sea turtles: control by thermal inhibition of activity. Nature 220:1338 9. Mrosovsky N. 1978. Orientation mechanisms of marine turtles. In: Schmidt-Koenig K, Keeton WT, editors. Animal migration, navigation, and homing. New York: Springer. p. 413 9. Mrosovsky N. 1980. Thermal biology of sea turtles. Am Zool 20:531 47. Mrosovsky N, Yntema CL. 1980. Temperature dependence of sexual differentiation in sea turtles: implications for conservation practices. Biol Conserv 18:271 80. Nagle RD, Lutz CL, Pyle AL. 2004. Overwintering in the nest by hatchling map turtles (Graptemys geographica). Can J Zool 82:1211 8. O Donoghue M, Boutin S. 1995. Does reproductive synchrony affect juvenile survival rates of northern mammals? Oikos 74:115 21. O Steen S, Janzen FJ. 1999. Embryonic temperature affects metabolic compensation and thyroid hormones in hatchling snapping turtles. Physiol Biochem Zool 72:520 33. Packard GC, Packard MJ. 1988. The physiological ecology of reptilian eggs and embryos. In: Gans C, Huey RB, editors. Biology of the Reptilia, Vol. 16. New York: Alan R. Liss, Inc. p. 524 605. Packard GC, Packard MJ, Miller K, Boardman TJ. 1987. Influence of moisture, temperature, and substrate on snapping turtle eggs and embryos. Ecology 68:983 93. Peterson CC, Kruegl A. 2005. Peaked temporal pattern of embryonic metabolism in an emydid turtle (Chrysemys picta). J Herp 39:678 81.

110 R.-J. Spencer and F. J. Janzen Pritchard PCH. 1969. Sea turtles of the Guianas. Bull Fla State Mus Biol Sci 13:85 140. Salmon M, Lohmann KJ. 1989. Orientation cues used by hatchling loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta) during their offshore migration. Ethology 83:215 28. Sexton OJ, Marion KR. 1974. Duration of incubation of Sceloporus undulatus eggs at constant temperature. Physiol Zool 47:91 8. Sih A, Moore RD. 1993. Delayed hatching of salamander eggs in response to enhanced larval predation risk. Am Nat 142:947 60. Speer-Blank T, Martin KLM. 2004. Hatching events in the California Grunion, Leuresthes tenuis. Copeia 2004:21 7. Spencer R. 2002. Experimentally testing nest site selection: fitness trade-offs and predation risk in turtles. Ecology 83:2136 44. Spencer R-J, Janzen FJ, Thompson MB. 2006. Counterintuitive density-dependent growth in a long-lived vertebrate after removal of nest predators. Ecology 87:3109 18. Spencer R-J, Thompson MB, Banks PB. 2001. Hatch or wait? A dilemma in reptilian incubation. Oikos 93:401 6. Stamper DL, Denver RJ, Licht P. 1990. Effects of thyroidal status on metabolism and growth of juvenile turtles, Pseudemys scripta elegans. Comp Biochem Physiol 96A:67 73. Testa JW. 2002. Does predation on neonates inherently select for earlier births? J Mammal 83:699 706. Thompson MB. 1988. Nest temperatures in the pleurodiran turtle, Emydura macquarii. Copeia 1988:996 1000. Thompson MB. 1989. Patterns of metabolism in embryonic reptiles. Resp Physiol 76:243 56. Thompson MB. 1997. Egg physiology and biology. In: Ackerman L, editor. The biology, husbandry, and healthcare of reptiles. New Jersey: TFH Publications Inc. p. 88 121. Tucker JK. 1997. Natural history notes on nesting, nests, and hatchling emergence in the red-eared slider turtle, Trachemys scripta elegans, in west-central Illinois. Ill Nat Hist Surv Biol Notes 140:1 13. Tucker JK. 1999. Environmental correlates of hatchling emergence in the red-eared turtle (Trachemys scripta elegans). Chelonian Conserv Biol 3:401 6. Tucker JK, Paukstis GL, Janzen FJ. 2008. Does predator swamping promote synchronous emergence of turtle hatchlings among nests? Behav Ecol 19:35 40. Vince MA. 1969. Embryonic communication, respiration and the synchronization of hatching. In: Hinde RA, editor. Bird vocalizations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 233 60. Vince MA, Chinn S. 1971. Effects of accelerated hatching on the initiation of standing and walking in the Japanese quail. Anim Behav 19:62 6. Vitt LJ. 1991. Ecology and life history of the scansorial arboreal lizard Plica plica (Iguanidae) in Amazonian Brazil. Can J Zool 69:504 11. Vonesh JR. 2000. Dipteran predation on the eggs of four Hyperolius frog species in western Uganda. Copeia 2000:560 6. Webb GJW, Choquenot D, Whitehead P. 1986. Nests, eggs and embryonic development of Carettochelys insculpta (Chelonia: Carettochelidae) from northern Australia. J Zool 1B:512 50. Warkentin KM. 1995. Adaptive plasticity in hatching age: a response to predation risk trade-offs. PNAS. 92:3507 3510. Warkentin KM. 2000. Wasp predation and wasp-induced hatching of redeyed treefrog eggs. Anim Behav 60:503 10. Weisrock DW, Janzen FJ. 1999. Thermal and fitness-related consequences of nest location in painted turtles (Chrysemys picta). Funct Ecol 13:94 101. Werner EE, Gilliam JF. 1984. The ontogenetic niche and species interactions in size structured populations. Ann Rev Ecol Syst 15:393 425. Zweifel RG. 1989. Long-term ecological studies on a population of painted turtles, Chrysemys picta, on Long Island, New York. Am Mus Novitates 2952:18 20.