Aggressiveness in king penguins in relation to reproductive status and territory location

Similar documents
King penguin brooding and defending a sub-antarctic skua chick

CHINSTRAP PENGUIN (PYGOSCELI$ ANTARCTICA): A FIELD EXPERIMENT

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

AVIAN PREDATION AT A SOUTHERN ROCKHOPPER PENGUIN COLONY ON STATEN ISLAND, ARGENTINA. Marcela Liljesthröm

Demography and breeding success of Falklands skua at Sea Lion Island, Falkland Islands

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp

AGE AT FIRST BREEDING AND CHANGE IN PLUMAGE OF KELP GULLS LARUS DOMINICANUS IN SOUTH AFRICA. R. J. M. CRAWFORD*, B. M. DYER* and L.

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

TIMING OF NEST RELIEF AND ITS EFFECT ON BREEDING SUCCESS IN ADELIE PENGUINS (PYGOSCELIS ADELIAE)

Alloparental feeding in the king penguin

Causes and Benefits of Chick Aggregations in Penguins

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

Ornithological Observations

Growth and Development. Embryonic development 2/22/2018. Timing of hatching. Hatching. Young birds and their parents

3. Chicks weigh 86 grams when they hatch and gain 100 grams a day until they are about 50 days old when they are ready to take care of itself.

GULLS (LARUS ARGENTATUS)

DAILY NEST ATTENDANCE AND BREEDING PERFORMANCE IN THE LITTLE PENGUIN EUDYPTULA MINOR AT PHILLIP ISLAND, AUSTRALIA

EXERCISE 14 Marine Birds at Sea World Name

THE EVOLUTION OF BEGGING

POPULATION DYNAMICS OF BREEDING SOUTH POLAR SKUAS OF UNKNOWN AGE ROBERT C. WOOD

BREEDING ROBINS AND NEST PREDATORS: EFFECT OF PREDATOR TYPE AND DEFENSE STRATEGY ON INITIAL VOCALIZATION PATTERNS

Polar Biol (1994) 14: Springer-Verlag 1994

BREEDING ECOLOGY OF THE LITTLE TERN, STERNA ALBIFRONS PALLAS, 1764 IN SINGAPORE

Anne-Marie Dulude a, Raymond McNeil a & Georg Baron a a Centre de recherches écologiques de Montréal, Département

ACTIVITY PATTERNS AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR OF NON-BREEDING ADkLIE PENGUINS

Aspects of the breeding biology of the southern rockhopper penguin Eudyptes c. chrysocome and new consideration on the intrinsic capacity of the A-egg

Spatial and temporal variation in the provisioning behaviour of female rockhopper penguins Eudyptes chrysocome filholi

Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey

A new application for transponders in studying penguins

DIURNAL TIME-ACTIVITY BUDGETS OF NESTING LEAST TERNS AND BLACK SKIMMERS. Melissa L. Leslie

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

Exploring Penguins through a research based information project. Includes information, writing pages, matrix for project.

You may use the information and images contained in this document for non-commercial, personal, or educational purposes only, provided that you (1)

Great Blue Heron Chick Development. Through the Stages

1. Adélie Penguins can mate for life or at least try to find the same mate every year.

OPTIMAL COLONY SIZE FOR LEAST TERNS: AN INTER- COLONY STUDY OF OPPOSING SELECTIVE PRESSURES BY PREDATORS

PENGUIN AND SOME OTHER PENGUINS. A. E. Bu}mE} AND A. J. WILLIAMS

Pair bond and breeding success in Blue Tits Parus caeruleus and Great Tits Parus major

BEHAVIOR OF THE ADkLIE PENGUIN CHICK

Rockhopper penguin (Eudyptes chrysocome filholi) foraging at Antipodes Islands

patch. The egg will be as snug and warm there as if it were in a sleeping bag. Penguin Chick By Betty Tatham Illustrated by Helen K.

PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE

Reduced availability of refuse and breeding output in a herring gull (Larus argentatus) colony

Pikas. Pikas, who live in rocky mountaintops, are not known to move across non-rocky areas or to

Wilson Bull., 94(2), 1982, pp

Vigilance Behaviour in Barnacle Geese

parental rearing capacities

EVALUATION OF A METHOD FOR ESTIMATING THE LAYING RATE OF BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS

AS91603 Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants & animals to their external environment

What is the date at which most chicks would have been expected to fledge?

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories

Crotophaga major (Greater Ani)

Conservation Management of Seabirds

Falkland Island Seabird Monitoring Programme Annual Report 2007/2008

DO BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS?

Effects of early incubation constancy on embryonic development: An experimental study in the herring gull Larus argentatus

Fun Penguin Facts. Instructions. All About Reading Extension Ideas: All About Spelling Extension Ideas:

Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) are breeding earlier at Creamer s Field Migratory Waterfowl Refuge, Fairbanks, AK

Avian Ecology: Life History, Breeding Seasons, & Territories

Anhinga anhinga (Anhinga or Snake-bird)

Tuning a nose to forage: Evidence for olfactory learning in a procellariiform seabird chicks

Health-dependent vulnerability to predation affects escape responses of unguarded chinstrap penguin chicks

Incubation feeding in snow buntings: female manipulation or indirect male parental care?

The Development of Behavior

Phenotypic selection on morphology at independence in the Chinstrap penguin Pygoscelis antarctica

Research Thesis. by Nathaniel J. Sackinger. The Ohio State University June 2013

Internship Report: Raptor Conservation in Bulgaria

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production

The Behavior of Glaucous-winged Gull Egg Cannibals

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production

Fun Penguin Facts. a reading and spelling review activity

BACKGROUND. About the Film. Adaptations. Introduction

Western Snowy Plover Recovery and Habitat Restoration at Eden Landing Ecological Reserve

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories

Male parental care and monogamy in snow buntings

Factors Influencing Local Recruitment in Tree Swallows, Tachycineta bicolor

Cape Hatteras National Seashore Resource Management Field Summary for July 15 July 21, 2010 (Bodie, Hatteras and Ocracoke Districts)

Piping Plover. Below: Note the color of the sand and the plover s back.

(199) THE HATCHING AND FLEDGING OF SOME COOT

From mountain to sea. A Survivor s Guide to Living with Urban Gulls

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

Prolonged foraging trips and egg desertion in little penguins (Eudyptula minor)

DO DIFFERENT CLUTCH SIZES OF THE TREE SWALLOW (Tachycineta bicolor)

WING AND PRIMARY GROWTH OF THE WANDERING ALBATROSS

C OLONIAL sea birds are characteristically long lived once they have fledged,

COMMISSION FOR THE CONSERVATION OF ANTARCTIC MARINE LIVING RESOURCES

Effects of a Pre-Molt Calcium and Low-Energy Molt Program on Laying Hen Behavior During and Post-Molt

Ardea herodias (Great Blue Heron)

The effect of testosterone injections on aggression and begging behaviour of black headed gull chicks (Larus ridibundus)

Factors Influencing Egg Production

Massachusetts Tern Census Form, 2012 Observers/Agency:

Temporal variation in leopard seal presence and predation near an Antarctic penguin rookery

PENGUINS. Marine Discovery Centre, Henley Beach, S.A. MDC 1

Emperor Penguin. Emperor Penguin : Assembly Instructions. Papercraft Mini-book / Assembly Instructions. Canon is a registered trademark of Canon Inc.

CISNET San Pablo Bay Avian Monitoring. Hildie Spautz, Nadav Nur & Julian Wood Point Reyes Bird Observatory

The Power of Observation Webcam technology helps students investigate the wonder of Adélie penguin breeding near Palmer Station, Antarctica

Short-term and Long-term Consequences of Predator Avoidance by Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor)

Assessing the effectiveness of temporarily excluding tourists from a Yellow-Eyed Penguin (Megadyptes antipodes) breeding beach

Transcription:

ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR,, 59, 813 821 doi:1.16/anbe.1999.1384, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Aggressiveness in king penguins in relation to reproductive status and territory location STEEVE D. CO TEu Centre d Eucologie et Physiologie Eunergétiques, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Strasbourg, France and Département de Biologie, Université de Sherbrooke, Québec (Received August 1999; accepted 24 November 1999; MS. number: A8336R) King penguins, Aptenodytes patagonicus, vigorously defend small territories in very dense colonies. The egg-laying season lasts approximately 4 months, but only pairs that reproduce during the first half of the period succeed in fledging a chick. I examined various factors affecting aggressiveness of king penguins during the breeding season and focused on the differences between central and peripheral territories. Pairs on peripheral territories experienced twice as many encounters with avian predators as did central birds. The vast majority of peripheral birds were late breeders, indicating that reproductive success was very low among penguins defending a territory on the edge of the colony. Time invested in territory defence and rate of agonistic encounters between breeding neighbours increased from the incubation to the brooding period. Parents gave most threat displays to territorial neighbours when the chick was very young and just before crèche formation. Distance to colony edge was not related to aggressiveness in incubating birds, however, the rate of pecking and flipper blows increased from the edge to the centre during brooding. In addition, aggressiveness of breeding penguins towards travelling birds trespassing into their territory increased with distance to edge. Early breeders were not more aggressive than late breeders but the proportion of time spent in territory defence increased with the number of days a bird had spent incubating. As expected, I did not detect any sex difference in aggressive behaviour. Birds occupying territories on the beach were generally more aggressive during the incubation period than those located on the valley sides. Reproductive status (incubating versus brooding) and territory location were the main factors explaining the various levels of aggressiveness observed in breeding king penguins. The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour The vast majority of seabirds nest in dense colonies where pairs vigorously defend a small breeding territory (Wittenberger & Hunt 1985). It has been proposed that productivity of seabirds may vary with location of the territory (Coulson 1968; Kharitonov & Siegel-Causey 1988). In several studies, birds occupying central territories have a higher reproductive success than peripheral birds, mainly because of a reduced risk of predation and flooding in the centre of the colony (Patterson 1965; Coulson 1968; Penney 1968; Tenaza 1971; Deixheimer & Southern 1974; Spurr 1975; Hutson 1977; Montevecchi 1978; Burger & Lesser 1979; Gochfeld 198; Wiklund 1982; Ainley et al. 1983; Burger & Gochfeld 199). However, some authors have reported no difference in reproductive success according to nest or territory location (Ryder & Ryder 1981; Giese 1996; Barbosa et al. 1997). In general, more predation attempts occur on the colony Correspondence and present address: S. D. Côté, Institute of Terrestrial Ecology, Banchory Research Station, Hill of Brathens, Glassel, Banchory, Kincardineshire, AB31 4BY, Scotland (email: s.cote@ceh.ac.uk). edge than in the centre (Taylor 1962; Penney 1968; Siegel-Causey & Hunt 1981; Emslie et al. 1995). In this situation, competition for territories is expected to be high and it is often assumed that birds nesting in the centre of a colony are more aggressive than peripheral birds in order to access high-quality central territories (Tenaza 1971; Pulliam 1973; Spurr 1974a; Hutson 1977; Kharitonov & Siegel-Causey 1988; Viñuela et al. 1995). Adélie penguins, Pygoscelis adeliae, that were aggressive towards an artificial penguin model, for example, occupied central territories and had a higher breeding success than less aggressive individuals (Spurr 1974a). Evidence for aggressive central territory holders, however, is rare and rather unconvincing, in part because it relies on the use of artificial bird models presented to territorial breeders (Spurr 1974a; Hutson 1977; Viñuela et al. 1995). Other factors may influence aggressiveness in breeding birds. Parental investment theory predicts that nest defence should increase with the advent of the breeding season because the fitness value of a chick is higher than that of an egg (Williams 1966; Trivers 1972; review in 3 3472//4813+9 $35./ 813 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour

814 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 59, 4 Montgomerie & Weatherhead 1988). As offspring age, renesting potential declines, the cost of replacing the brood increases and the difference between survival probabilities of parents and young is reduced, thus increasing the future fitness benefits of the parents (Barash 1975; Andersson et al. 198; Burger 198; Montgomerie & Weatherhead 1988). According to these arguments, the value of a chick and the willingness of the parents to defend it should increase as chicks approach fledging. However, some authors have found that aggression is higher at hatching, when chicks are more vulnerable (Burger 1981, 1983; Burger & Gochfeld 199). Also, extrinsic factors such as habitat type may affect the level of aggressiveness observed (Kharitonov & Siegel-Causey 1988; Burger & Gochfeld 1992). In king penguins, Aptenodytes patagonicus, the egglaying season starts in mid-november and lasts about 4 months, 2 3 months longer than in the majority of other penguin species (Weimerskirch et al. 1992; Jouventin & Mauget 1996). Only those adults that breed in the first half of the breeding season, however, can fledge a chick (Weimerskirch et al. 1992; van Heezik et al. 1994; Olsson 1996; Brodin et al. 1998). Survival of chicks hatched after February is very low because they do not have time to attain the critical body mass (1 12 kg) necessary to survive the subantarctic winter (Cherel & Le Maho 1985; Weimerskirch et al. 1992; van Heezik et al. 1994). Chicks are fed infrequently in winter or may not be fed at all for as long as 5 months (Cherel & Le Maho 1985; Weimerskirch et al. 1992). Consequently, the fitness value of chicks and the willingness of parents to defend them should be higher for chicks hatched by early breeders than for chicks hatched by late breeders. King penguin pairs vigorously defend a territory of approximately.5 m 2 against breeding neighbours and intruders that approach within pecking distance (Barrat 1976; Le Maho et al. 1993). The pair incubates a single egg directly on their feet and fasts both when incubating and brooding a chick (Stonehouse 19; Barrat 1976). Parents alternate nest duties every 5 22 days (Barrat 1976; Weimerskirch et al. 1992). Because both sexes actively defend the territory during incubation and brooding and are of very similar body size (Barrat 1976), I did not expect to find any difference in aggressiveness between males and females. I was interested in determining what factors affect aggressiveness in breeding king penguins. Particularly, I tested the hypotheses that reproductive success is higher for central territories than peripheral territories and that individuals occupying central locations are more aggressive than birds on the edges of the colony. Also, I assessed whether breeding date, time spent incubating, sex and habitat type affected aggressiveness. Finally, I tested whether aggressiveness increases with offspring age (from egg to fledgling), as predicted by the parental investment theory. METHODS This study was conducted in the breeding colony of la Baie du Marin (4 pairs) on Possession Island, Crozet Archipelago (46 25 S, 51 45 E; Weimerskirch et al. 1992). Breeding birds were associated in one single colony covering approximately 25 m 2. I located pairs on their breeding territory before laying, and sprayed them at 1 2 m with a dye. The day after laying, a plastic Darwick tag was attached around the flipper of the incubating bird and the egg was marked with a permanent marker. Because the male performs the first incubation shift in king penguins (Stonehouse 19; Barrat 1976; Weimerskirch et al. 1992), the male was marked first. The female was marked during her first incubation shift (i.e. 2 3 weeks after laying). As part of another study, some marked birds (42%) were later untagged and a number was painted on their chest with Nyanzol-D branding dye (Giese 1996). Interventions in the colony were minimized to reduce disturbance to the birds. I knew the exact day of laying for each marked female, and pairs were observed daily during the entire incubation period to time all incubation shifts. In king penguins, hatching occurs on average 53 days after the initiation of incubation (Weimerskirch et al. 1992). Early breeders lay their egg in late November early December (see Weimerskirch et al. 1992). Those that laid after the first hatching was observed, or those that were still incubating an egg after February, were considered late breeders (van Heezik et al. 1994). I conducted a survey of more than 43 breeding pairs in the colony from 27 February to 1 March 1998 to determine the proportion of early (those brooding a chick) and late (those still incubating an egg) breeders in the periphery and centre of the colony. In king penguins, the distinction between early and late breeders is important because less than 5% of late breeders fledge chicks, compared with more than 5% for early breeders (Weimerskirch et al. 1992; Olsson 1996). Following Ainley et al. (1983), Emslie et al. (1995) and Barbosa et al. (1997), I defined the periphery as the area situated within approximately 2 m of the colony edge. Birds situated more than 2 m from the periphery have at least two or three circles of conspecific neighbours occupying territories in all directions around them and, therefore, are not the first birds to interact with predators attempting to find a passage into the colony (Ainley et al. 1983; Emslie et al. 1995). I estimated that approximately % of the colony area was occupied by the periphery, which represented approximately 8 breeding pairs. I used 15 min focal animal sampling to study king penguins aggressive behaviour (Altmann 1974). A total of 178 focal observations were conducted from 26 January to 2 March 1998. Observations covered the complete daylight period, from to 21 hours, and were carried out from outside the colony at distances of 1 3 m. I used five different blinds to conduct observations at close range and a 22 wide-angle spotting scope to observe individuals situated at distances greater than 15 m. Territorial birds were regularly spaced throughout the colony and all focal penguins were occupying a breeding territory adjacent to other territorial birds. The number of neighbours situated at a pecking distance from the focal bird ranged from two to seven (X SD=4.6 1.3, N=178) and distance between neighbours had a mean SD of 49.4 8.9 cm (N=67). I

CO TEu: TERRITORY DEFENCE IN KING PENGUINS 815 conducted 72 (4%) focal observations on birds selected randomly among marked birds. All other focal samples (N=16) were undertaken on unmarked and untouched birds selected randomly in the colony. I used natural features present in the colony and an artificial grid system to ensure that each unmarked bird was sampled once, and that only the male or the female on the same territory was observed. In addition, I did not sample the behaviour of neighbours because data collected from individuals contesting the same boundary are not likely to be independent. In each focal sample, the time of the day, weather and general habitat of the territory (on the beach on level ground or on the sides of the valley with a 1 3 rocky slope) were noted. Because penguins were regularly spaced throughout the colony, the density of territorial breeders was similar in the two habitats. Also, I sampled equivalent numbers of penguins near to and far from the periphery on both the beach and the valley sides. At the beginning of each focal observation, ambient temperature was measured (to the nearest degree) and wind speed assigned as: no wind, light, moderate, strong or very strong. During the focal sample, I recorded all agonistic interactions with neighbours and intruders moving through the territory. I also noted all instances of penguin movements on the territory that did not elicit aggressive behaviour. I estimated visually the distance of the focal bird to the colony edge and to the closest penguin trail to the nearest.5 m. Before starting the study and regularly afterwards, I verified my distance estimations with a 1 m ruler. King penguin activities were classified as: (1) territory defence: aggressive behaviour including beak pointing (no vocalization, beak closed, body stretched out), gaping (pointing but with bill open and vocalizing, body stretched out), pecking and flipper blows (Spurr 1974b; Challet et al. 1994); (2) maintenance: mainly preening but also included stretching, moving and shaking the head, slow and rapid flipper flapping, tail wagging, vocalizing, scratching etc. (Ainley 1974; Challet et al. 1994); (3) rest: standing, bill pointed forward with no head movement (Challet et al. 1994); (4) sleep: with head twisted and beak tucked down underneath a flipper (Buchet et al. 1986); and (5) chick-related behaviour: regurgitating food, preening the chick, singing before feeding, any movements to restrain chick from moving away. Penguins arriving from and leaving for the ocean do not move randomly into the colony, but seem to use trails just outside the colony for most of their terrestrial movements (Côté & Dewasmes 1999). Because trails are situated mainly on the edges of the colony, distance to colony edge and distance to closest penguin trail were strongly correlated (Spearman s rank correlation: r S =.91, N=178, P<.1). Thus, I only considered distance to colony edge when analysing the effects of territory location on aggressiveness. Chick size was estimated during focal samples according to the chick s relative height compared to the parent and to the approximate surface exposed when lying, head underneath the brooding pouch of the parent. I used five classes of relative body size and tested the rationale of these categories by following the growth of 54 chicks of known age. The average age in each of the five categories corresponded closely to the first 5 weeks of life, that is, the period until emancipation from the parent (31 4 days) (Barrat 1976; Weimerskirch et al. 1992; unpublished data). Chicks grow from about 43 g during their first week to 3.2 kg at 5 weeks (Barrat 1976). During focal observations, I recorded all instances of predators walking within possible pecking distance of the focal individual and noted whether the focal penguin threatened, attacked (physical contact) or ignored the predator. I observed a total of 65 passages of Antarctic and subantarctic giant petrels (Macronectes giganteus and M. halli, respectively), subantarctic skua, Catharacta lonnbergi, kelp gull, Larus dominicanus, and lesser sheathbill, Chionis minor, in 33 focal samples (19% of total), none resulting in successful predation. Statistical Analyses I separated threat displays involving no body contact (i.e. beak pointing and gaping) from aggressive interactions with body contact (pecking and flipper blows) in the analyses because I considered that interactions with body contact were more aggressive than encounters without contact (see also Penney 1968; Spurr 1974b; Jouventin 1982). Focal individuals with six or seven neighbours were grouped in one category of greater than versus equal to six neighbours due to small sample size. Time spent defending the territory was arcsine squareroot transformed to meet normality. Rates of threat display and aggressive interactions with body contact were not normal despite transformation, and therefore I used nonparametric tests in the analyses involving these variables. I calculated the proportion of nonterritorial birds walking through a territory that were threatened or attacked by the focal individual, and compared these values to the distance to colony edge. Because number of neighbours affected aggressiveness and varied with distance to colony edge in brooding birds (see Results), I used the Kendall rank partial correlation coefficient, controlling for the number of neighbours, to test the effect of distance to edge on aggressive behaviour (Siegel & Castellan 1988). All other variables were not related to number of neighbours. Statistical procedures followed Sokal & Rohlf (1981) and all probability values were two-tailed, with significance level set at α=.5. Averages are reported with one standard error. RESULTS About 69% of king penguins on the periphery of the colony were still incubating in late February-early March. Late breeders were thus much more common on the colony edge than in the centre (G test: G 1 =127.9, N=4324, P<.1). In contrast, only % of central birds were incubating at that time, indicating that most central territories were occupied by early breeders. In addition, peripheral penguins had about twice as many interactions with predators (24.4% of 9 focal samples) than

816 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 59, 4 Table 1. Variation in individual rate of aggressive behaviour (behaviours/h±se) according to reproductive status in king penguins from Crozet Islands Incubating N Brooding N U P Threat displays* 59.±4.3 81 111.8±8.8 97 2473 <.1 Body contact interactions 15.2±4.9 81 25.7±4.2 97 31.3 *Beak pointing and gaping. Pecking and flipper blows. Mann Whitney U test. central birds (12.5% of 88 focal samples) (G test: G 1 =4.27, N=178, P=.4). Ambient temperature and time of day were not related to either proportion of time spent in territory defence (linear regressions: N=178, all NS) or rate of aggressive interactions (Spearman s rank correlations: N=178, all NS). Similarly, wind speed did not affect the proportion of time spent in territory defence (ANOVA: F 3,173 =.43, P=.73) or rate of aggressive interactions (Kruskal Wallis test: H 3 =3.5, N=177, P=.38). No measure of aggressiveness differed between marked and unmarked birds (Mann Whitney U tests: N 1 =73, N 2 =16, all NS). Therefore, all focal samples were pooled in the following analyses. Penguins brooding a chick spent 18.7 1.5% of observation time in territorial defence compared with 11.5 1.1% for birds incubating (t test: t 176 = 3.74, P=.2). Rates of threat display and of aggressive interactions with body contact were also much greater in brooding birds than in incubating birds (Table 1). The number of neighbours on territories close enough to allow interactions involving body contact did not affect the proportion of time spent in territory defence (ANOVA: F 4,76 =.43, P=.78), rate of threat displays (Kruskal Wallis test: H 4 =3.35, N=81, P=.5), or rate of aggressive interactions with body contact (H 4 =3.13, N=81, P=.54) in incubating birds. For brooding birds, however, those with six or more neighbours spent proportionally more time defending their territory than birds with two neighbours (ANOVA: F 4,92 =2.83, P=.3; Fisher post-hoc least significant difference). Also, the rate of threat displays (Kruskal Wallis test: H 4 =14.85, N=97, P=.5) and the rate of aggressive interactions with body contact (H 4 =16.88, N=97, P=.2) increased with number of neighbours in brooding birds. Distance to colony edge was not related to the proportion of time spent in territory defence (Spearman rank correlation: r S =.8, N=81, P=.46), nor to rates of threat displays (r S =.6, N=81, P=.57), or aggressive interactions with body contact (r S =.1, N=81, P=.92) in incubating birds. Not surprisingly, penguins in central territories had more neighbours than peripheral birds (r S =.21, N=97, P=.38). For this reason, and mainly because the number of neighbours affected aggressiveness of brooding penguins, I controlled for the number of neighbours (N) when testing the effect of distance to edge (E) on aggressiveness in brooding birds. Proportion of time spent in territory defence (D) (Kendall rank partial correlation coefficient: r DE. N =.3, N=97, P=.62) and rate of threat displays (T) (r TE. N =.6, N=97, P=.37) were not affected by distance to edge in brooding birds. Rate of aggressive interactions with body contact (B), however, increased towards the centre of the colony in birds brooding a chick (r BE. N =.16, N=97, P=.17). In addition, the proportion of intruding penguins attacked when trespassing into the territory of focal individuals increased with distance to edge (Spearman s rank correlation: incubating birds, r S =.28, N=, P=.3; brooding birds, r S =.33, N=69, P=.7; Fig. 1). An alternative explanation could be that central birds are more aggressive than peripheral birds simply because they are less often disturbed by intruders and could therefore invest more time and energy in territory defence. This scenario, however, appears unlikely because the number of intruders did not vary between central and peripheral territories (Mann Whitney U test: U=3686.5, N 1 =9, N 2 =88, P=.43). Chick age did not affect the proportion of time spent in territory defence (ANOVA: F 4,92 =1.82, P=.13). The rate of threat displays varied significantly with chick age (Kruskal Wallis test: H 4 =1.6, N=97, P=.39) but the rate of aggressive interactions with body contact did not (H 4 =5.35, N=97, P=.25; Fig. 2). Parents appeared to point and gape more when the chick was very young (week 1) and just before crèche formation (week 5; Fig. 2). Aggressive interactions with body contact followed the same temporal pattern, although the relationship was not significant (Fig. 2). Proportion of time spent defending the territory during incubation was not related to breeding date, indicating that early breeders were not more aggressive than late breeders (Spearman s rank correlation: r S =.18, N=34, P=.31). Also, rate of threat displays (r S =.21, N=34, P=.24) and rate of aggressive interactions with body contact (r S =.13, N=34, P=.46) did not vary with breeding date. Proportion of time spent in territory defence increased with the number of days the bird had spent incubating, both sexes combined (F 1,31 =4.99, R 2 =.14, P=.3; Fig. 3). On the other hand, I did not find any relation between the rates of aggressive behaviour and the number of days spent incubating (Spearman s rank correlation: threats: r S =.3, N=32, P=.88; aggressive interactions with body contact: r S =.7, N=32, P=.71), indicating that although incubating birds did not interact more often, interactions lasted longer on average towards the end of incubation shifts.

CO TEu: TERRITORY DEFENCE IN KING PENGUINS 817 Percentage of travelling penguins attacked 1 8 4 1 8 4 Incubating 3 6 9 12 15 18 Aggressive behaviours/h 15 16 1 27 18 9 16 3 1 2 3 4 5 Chick age (week) Figure 2. Individual rate of aggressive behaviour of brooding king penguins as a function of chick age on Crozet Islands. Threat displays ( ) include beak pointing and gaping; body contact interactions ( ) include pecking and flipper blows. Sample sizes of focal individuals for each week are shown above bars. %Time in territory defence 5 4 3 Brooding 5 1 15 25 Distance to edge (m) Figure 1. Percentage of travelling king penguins attacked by territorial birds according to reproductive status and distance to colony edge at Crozet Islands. Each point refers to a different focal animal. Male and female king penguins spent a similar amount of time in territory defence while incubating (t test: t 32 =.46, P=.65) and brooding (t 36 =.55, P=.59). In addition, they did not differ in rates of aggressive behaviour (Fig. 4). Incubating penguins occupying territories on the flat beach spent 13.3% of their time in territory defence compared with 9.1% for birds on the sides of the valley (t test: t 79 =1.95, P=.5). Birds incubating on the beach also had a higher rate of aggressive interactions with body contact than birds on territories on the sides of the valley, but did not display more threats (Table 2). Habitat type did not affect the aggressive behaviour of brooding birds. Penguins on the beach and on the sides of the valley spent similar amounts of time in territory defence (t test: t 95 =.65, P=.52) and had the same rates of threat display 1 5 1 15 Days spent incubating Figure 3. Effect of number of days spent incubating on proportion of time spent in territory defence in king penguins of Crozet Islands. and aggressive interactions with body contact while brooding (Table 2). DISCUSSION In all penguin species but the king penguin (Brodin et al. 1998), the reproductive cycle is completed within 1 year (Williams 1995; Pütz et al. 1998). Although the date of arrival in the colony varies, most adult birds are competing for access to the best territories at approximately the same time (Coulson 1968; Kharitonov & Siegel-Causey 1988; Trivelpiece & Trivelpiece 199). In king penguins, a pair needs 14 16 months to fledge a chick, thus reproducing early one year precludes early reproduction the following year (Weimerskirch et al. 1992; Olsson 1996). Inevitably, following a successful reproductive episode one year, the pair will either skip a year or breed late the

818 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 59, 4 Aggressive behaviours/h 9 75 45 3 15 15 125 1 75 5 25 19 15 18 Threat displays Brooding Body contact interactions next year and be very unlikely to fledge a chick (Weimerskirch et al. 1992; van Heezik et al. 1994; Olsson 1996). Therefore, all birds do not necessarily compete in the same period for access to territories because the egg-laying period extends over 4 months. An experienced late breeder, for example, could arrive in the colony when many birds, both experienced and inexperienced, are already hatching their chicks. In my study, early breeders (high reproductive success) occupied most central territories (8%) while late breeders (low reproductive success) occurred mainly on the edges (69%), suggesting that the first birds to arrive chose the central locations and were gradually surrounded by late-arriving pairs. Adélie penguins that move from a peripheral nest to a central nest 19 Incubating Figure 4. Individual rate of aggressive behaviour of male ( )and female ( ) king penguins incubating an egg or brooding a chick on Crozet Islands. Threat displays include beak pointing and gaping; body contact interactions include pecking and flipper blows. No difference between males and females was detected (Mann Whitney U test, all NS). Sample sizes are shown above bars. 15 18 the following year have a higher breeding success than individuals that stay on the edge, while individuals changing from the centre to the periphery have a lower success than those that remain in the centre (Spurr 1975). Peripheral territories in my study appeared of lesser quality than central territories because chicks on the edges were about twice as susceptible to predator attacks than chicks in central locations (see also Penney 1968; Emslie et al. 1995). Predation is the main cause of mortality in penguin chicks (Taylor 1962; Spurr 1975; Hunter 1991) and Emslie et al. (1995) showed that predation and predation attempts were four to eight times more frequent at colony edges than in the centre in Adélie and Gentoo, Pygoscelis papua, penguin colonies. Furthermore, philopatry appears low in king penguins and cannot explain the choice of territory location. Individuals likely change territories and have different neighbours every year because the divorce rate is very high in king penguins (approximately 8%; Olsson 1998; Bried et al. 1999) and the same birds alternate early and late breeding attempts (Weimerskirch et al. 1992). In such a situation where central territories appear to afford better protection against predators and provide higher breeding success, should experienced but late-arriving birds try to access a central territory by displacing a subordinate bird? The answer to this is two-fold. First, the territory, regardless of its location, does not have the same value for a bird arriving to breed and one already brooding a chick because the parental investment necessary to replace a chick is much higher than that to replace an egg (Montgomerie & Weatherhead 1988). King penguins that lose their chick cannot replace it during the same year (Weimerskirch et al. 1992). Because the value of the territory is much higher for birds already brooding a chick compared with late-arriving birds, regardless of experience or aggressiveness, brooding birds should be very reluctant to abandon their territory (Spurr 1974a, b; Montgomerie & Weatherhead 1988). The very high level of aggressiveness observed in breeding king penguins (up to 5 interactions per bird per h; Le Maho et al. 1993; Challet et al. 1994) suggests that although fights incur high energetic costs (Hogstad 1987), the benefits of defending a territory must be very high (Montgomerie & Weatherhead 1988). The observed increase in aggressiveness from incubation to brooding (Table 1) can be explained by the higher fitness value of a chick as proposed by parental investment theory (Burger 1981; Siegel- Causey & Hunt 1981; Kilpi 1987; Viñuela et al. 1995; Amat et al. 1996). Similarly, Challet et al. (1994) obtained a three-fold increase in territory defence of king penguins, comparing the incubation and the brooding periods. Chinstrap penguins, Pygoscelis antarctica, also defend chicks more strongly than eggs (Viñuela et al. 1995). There is often a marked behavioural change when a penguin chick hatches, and aggressiveness increases importantly (Spurr 1974a; Derksen 1977). Although the proportion of time spent defending did not change with chick age in king penguins, parents gave more threat displays during the first week after hatching, probably because the chick was very vulnerable at this time (Fig. 2; see also Spurr 1974a; Andersson et al. 198; Burger 1981;

CO TEu: TERRITORY DEFENCE IN KING PENGUINS 819 Table 2. Effect of habitat type and reproductive status on individual rate of aggressive behaviour (behaviours/h±se) in breeding king penguins from Crozet Islands Beach N Valley sides N U P Incubating Threat displays* 15.1±1.3 47 14.3±1.8 34 716.43 Body contact interactions 5.1±2.1 47 1.9±.5 34 585.4 Brooding Threat displays* 27.6±3.6 48 28.±2.6 49 174.46 Body contact interactions 7.6±1.7 48 5.3±1.3 49 1127.72 *Beak pointing and gaping. Pecking and flipper blows. Mann Whitney U test. Burger & Gochfeld 199). The increase in threat displays observed at the time of crèche formation correspond to the period when the chick reaches its highest fitness value of the guarding phase (Montgomerie & Weatherhead 1988). Also, the increase in aggressiveness with the number of neighbours observed in brooding birds but not in incubating birds indicates the greater propensity of brooding penguins to engage in agonistic behaviours. Second, based solely on the parental investment theory, there should be no difference in aggressiveness between birds raising a chick in the centre and on the edge of the colony. However, I observed that individuals in the centre (controlling for the number of immediate neighbours) performed more aggressive behaviours towards neighbours than did peripheral birds. This indicates a greater motivation to territory defence in central birds (see also Spurr 1974a, b), even though the proportion of time spent defending the territory was not related to territory location. Individuals in central locations were also more aggressive towards intruders than those on the edges (Fig. 1). Viñuela et al. (1995) reported similar results in chinstrap penguins exposed to human intruders. It would therefore be more difficult for a recently arrived king penguin to displace a central bird than a peripheral one, because the former would be more aggressive towards direct neighbours and intruders than the latter. In addition, a bird arriving would face the attacks of all other territorial breeders while making its way to the centre (Ainley et al. 1983; Côté & Dewasmes 1999). Even if central territories appear more costly to defend in king penguins, it is possible that early breeders select them because they provide the chick better protection against predator attacks and flooding. Finally, aggressiveness did not vary between early and late breeders, indicating that reproductive status (incubating versus brooding) and territory location were the main factors explaining the various levels of aggressiveness observed. King penguins spent more time in territory defence and the average duration of agonistic behaviours was longer as the number of days spent incubating increased (Fig. 3). The diminution of body reserves during the incubation shift (Cherel et al. 1994) could increase the stress level of a bird and induce higher levels of aggressiveness. In addition, penguins tend to vocalize more towards the end of a shift, a behaviour that normally elicits aggressiveness of neighbours (Stonehouse 19; Barrat 1976). In their study of video-recorded king penguins, Challet et al. (1994) reported no difference in territory defence between the onset and the end of an incubation shift. However, they monitored only five birds with a recorder operating nine times slower than real time, which might have precluded detection of subtle differences in aggressive behaviour. As predicted, I did not observe any difference in the aggressive behaviour of male and female king penguins (Fig. 4). On the other hand, male Adélie (Spurr 1974a) and chinstrap (Moreno et al. 1995; Viñuela et al. 1995) penguins are more aggressive than females during pair formation, probably because they have to fend off other males while building a nest and attracting a female. Aggressiveness of males and females does not differ in Adélie penguins following egg laying (Spurr 1974a). However, the situation is different in king penguins because first, they do not build a nest and second, pairs form on the beach and then establish the territory together (Barrat 1976). During the incubation period, pairs established on the flat beach were more aggressive than those using the sides of the valley, irrespective of distance to colony edge. There were more penguins walking on the beach than on the valley sides and this intense circulation could have induced the higher level of aggressiveness observed. The absence of a relationship between all measures of aggressiveness and habitat type in brooding birds is somehow puzzling. I suggest that the level of aggressiveness observed in brooding birds was too high (average of 19% of time spent in territory defence and 138 aggressive interactions per bird per h) to detect a difference among habitats. Birds raising a chick in both habitats probably reached a threshold of aggressive interactions at which point it became energetically too costly to respond with an increase in aggressiveness (Hogstad 1987). Aggressiveness increased from incubation to brooding, supporting the parental investment theory. Also, central birds were more aggressive than individuals on the edge of the colony and experienced a higher reproductive success. Aggressiveness of breeders, however, was not directly compared to survival of chicks. To understand fully the relationship between aggressiveness and reproduction in seabirds, further research should follow individually marked animals to measure aggressive behaviour

8 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 59, 4 and long-term reproductive success according to various territory locations. Acknowledgments This study was carried out under program 119- Stockage, mobilisation et transfert d acides gras chez les animaux polaires of the Institut Français pour la Recherche et la Technologie Polaire (France, IFRTP). I am indebted to René Groscolas, director of this programme, for the opportunity to work at Crozet. I also thank all the members of the R. Groscolas and Y. Le Maho research teams for capturing penguins: F. Decrock, C. Fayolle, J.-C. Fournier, C. Gilly, N. Lambert, E. Leroy, J.-P. Robin and A. Rosinski. Capture and tagging procedures were approved by the Animal Care Committee of the IFRTP (program 119). Financial support was provided by IFRTP, the Fonds pour la Formation des chercheurs et Aide à la Recherche (Québec, Canada) and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada. I thank the Terres Australes et Antarctiques Françaises for logistic support. I very much appreciated the discussion and critical comments of M. Festa-Bianchet, Y. Handrich, W. J. King, J.-P. Robin and S. Shaffer on the manuscript. This is contribution no. 131 of the Groupe de recherche en écologie, nutrition et énergétique, Université de Sherbrooke. References Ainley, D. G. 1974. The comfort behaviour of Adélie and other penguins. Behaviour, 5, 16 51. Ainley, D. G., LeResche, R. E. & Sladen, W. J. L. 1983. Breeding Biology of the Adélie Penguin. Berkeley: University of California Press. Altmann, J. 1974. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour, 49, 227 267. Amat, J. A., Carrascal, L. M. & Moreno, J. 1996. Nest defence by chinstrap penguins Pygoscelis antarctica in relation to offspring number and age. Journal of Avian Biology, 27, 177 179. Andersson, M., Wiklund, C. G. & Rundgren, H. 198. Parental defence of offspring: a model and an example. Animal Behaviour, 28, 536 542. Barash, D. P. 1975. Evolutionary aspects of parental behavior: distraction behavior of the alpine accentor. Wilson Bulletin, 87, 367 373. Barbosa, A., Moreno, J., Potti, J. & Merino, S. 1997. Breeding group size, nest position and breeding success in the chinstrap penguin. Polar Biology, 18, 41 414. Barrat, A. 1976. Quelques aspects de la biologie et de l écologie du manchot royal (Aptenodytes patagonicus) des îles Crozet. Commission Nationale Française de Recherche Antarctique, 4, 9 51. Bried, J., Jiguet, F. & Jouventin, P. 1999. Why do Aptenodytes penguins have high divorce rates? Auk, 116, 54 512. Brodin, A., Olsson, O. & Clark, C. W. 1998. Modeling the breeding cycle of long-lived birds: why do king penguins try to breed late? Auk, 115, 767 771. Buchet, C., Dewasmes, G. & Le Maho, Y. 1986. An electrophysiological and behavioral study of sleep in emperor penguins under natural ambient conditions. Physiology and Behavior, 38, 331 335. Burger, J. 198. Territory size differences in relation to reproductive stage and type of intruder in herring gulls (Larus argentatus). Auk, 97, 733 741. Burger, J. 1981. Aggressive behaviour of black skimmers (Rynchops niger). Behaviour, 76, 7 222. Burger, J. 1983. Competition between two species of nesting gulls: on the importance of timing. Behavioral Neuroscience, 97, 492 51. Burger, J. & Gochfeld, M. 199. The Black Skimmer: Social Dynamics of a Colonial Species. New York: Columbia University Press. Burger, J. & Gochfeld, M. 1992. Experimental evidence for aggressive antipredator behavior in black skimmers (Rynchops niger). Aggressive Behavior, 18, 241 248. Burger, J. & Lesser, F. 1979. Breeding behavior and success in salt marsh common tern colonies. Bird-banding, 5, 322 337. Challet, E., Bost, C.-A., Handrich, Y., Gendner, J.-P. & Le Maho, Y. 1994. Behavioural time budget of breeding king penguins (Aptenodytes patagonica). Journal of Zoology, 233, 669 681. Cherel, Y. & Le Maho, Y. 1985. Five months of fasting in king penguin chicks: body mass loss and fuel metabolism. American Journal of Physiology, 249, R387 R392. Cherel, Y., Gilles, J., Handrich, Y. & Le Maho, Y. 1994. Nutrient reserve dynamics and energetics during long-term fasting in the king penguin (Aptenodytes patagonicus). Journal of Zoology, 234, 1 12. Côté, S. D. & Dewasmes, G. 1999. Do sleeping king penguins influence the movement of conspecifics through a colony? Polar Biology, 22, 13 16. Coulson, J. C. 1968. Differences in the quality of birds nesting in the centre and on the edges of a colony. Nature, 217, 478 479. Deixheimer, M. & Southern, W. E. 1974. Breeding success relative to nest location and density in ring-billed gull colonies. Wilson Bulletin, 86, 288 29. Derksen, D. V. 1977. A quantitative analysis of the incubation behavior of the Adélie penguin. Auk, 94, 552 566. Emslie, S. D., Karnovsky, N. & Trivelpiece, W. 1995. Avian predation at penguin colonies on King George Island, Antarctica. Wilson Bulletin, 17, 317 327. Giese, M. 1996. Effects of human activity on Adelie penguin Pygoscelis adeliae breeding success. Biological Conservation, 75, 157 164. Gochfeld, M. 198. Timing of breeding and chick mortality in central and peripheral nests of Magellanic penguins. Auk, 97, 191 193. van Heezik, Y. M., Seddon, P. J., Cooper, J. & Plös,A.L.1994. Interrelationships between breeding frequency, timing and outcome in king penguins Aptenodytes patagonicus: are king penguins biennial breeders? Ibis, 136, 279 284. Hogstad, O. 1987. It is expensive to be dominant. Auk, 14, 333 336. Hunter, S. 1991. The impact of avian predator-scavengers on king penguin Aptenodytes patagonicus chicks at Marion Island. Ibis, 133, 343 35. Hutson, G. D. 1977. Agonistic display and spacing in the blackheaded gull, Larus ridibundus. Animal Behaviour, 25, 765 773. Jouventin, P. 1982. Visual and Vocal Signals in Penguins, Their Evolution and Adaptive Characters. Berlin: Parey. Jouventin, P. & Mauget, R. 1996. The endocrine basis of the reproductive cycle in the king penguin (Aptenodytes patagonicus). Journal of Zoology, 238, 665 678. Kharitonov, S. P. & Siegel-Causey, D. 1988. Colony formation in seabirds. Current Ornithology, 5, 223 272. Kilpi, M. 1987. Do herring gulls (Larus argentatus) invest more in offspring defense as the breeding season advances? Ornis Fennica, 64, 16. Le Maho, Y., Gendner, J.-P., Challet, Eu., Bost, C.-A., Gilles, J., Verdon, C., Plumeré, C., Robin, J.-P. & Handrich, Y. 1993. Undisturbed breeding penguins as indicators of changes in marine resources. Marine Ecology Progress Series, 95, 1 6.

CO TEu: TERRITORY DEFENCE IN KING PENGUINS 821 Montevecchi, W. A. 1978. Nest site selection and its survival value among laughing gulls. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology, 4, 143 161. Montgomerie, R. D. & Weatherhead, P. J. 1988. Risks and rewards of nest defence by parent birds. Quaterly Review of Biology, 63, 167 187. Moreno, J., Bustamante, J. & Viñuela, J. 1995. Nest maintenance and stone theft in the chinstrap penguin (Pygoscelis antarctica). 1. Sex roles and effects on fitness. Polar Biology, 15, 533 54. Olsson, O. 1996. Seasonal effects of timing and reproduction in the king penguin: a unique breeding style. Journal of Avian Biology, 27, 7 14. Olsson, O. 1998. Divorce in king penguins: asynchrony, expensive fat storing and ideal free mate choice. Oikos, 83, 574 581. Patterson, I. J. 1965. Timing and spacing of broods in the blackheaded gull Larus ridibundus. Ibis, 17, 433 459. Penney, R. L. 1968. Territorial and social behaviour in the Adélie penguin. In: Antarctic Bird Studies (Ed. by O. L. Austin, Jr), pp. 83 131. Washington, DC: American Geophysical Union. Pulliam, H. R. 1973. On the advantages of flocking. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 38, 419 422. Pütz, K., Wilson, R. P., Charrassin, J.-B., Raclot, T., Lage, J., Le Maho, Y., Kierspel, M. A. M., Culik, B. M. & Adelung, D. 1998. Foraging strategy of king penguins (Aptenodytes patagonicus) during summer at the Crozet Islands. Ecology, 79, 195 1921. Ryder, P. L. & Ryder, J. P. 1981. Reproductive performance of ring-billed gulls in relation to nest location. Condor, 83, 57. Siegel, S. & Castellan, N. J., Jr. 1988. Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill. Siegel-Causey, D. & Hunt, G. L., Jr. 1981. Colonial defense behavior in double-crested and pelagic cormorants. Auk, 98, 522 531. Sokal, R. R. & Rohlf, F. J. 1981. Biometry. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman. Spurr, E. B. 1974a. Individual differences in aggressiveness of Adélie penguins. Animal Behaviour, 22, 611 616. Spurr, E. B. 1974b. Communication in the Adélie penguin. In: The Biology of Penguins (Ed. by B. Stonehouse), pp. 449 51. London: Macmillan. Spurr, E. B. 1975. Breeding of the Adélie penguin Pygoscelis adeliae at Cape Bird. Ibis, 117, 324 338. Stonehouse, B. 19. The king penguin Aptenodytes patagonica of South Georgia. I. Breeding behaviour and development. Scientific Reports Falkland Islands Dependencies Survey, 23, 1 81. Taylor, R. H. 1962. The Adelie penguin Pygoscelis adeliae at Cape Royds. Ibis, 14, 176 4. Tenaza, R. 1971. Behavior and nesting success relative to nest location in Adélie penguins (Pygoscelis adeliae). Condor, 73, 81 92. Trivelpiece, W. Z. & Trivelpiece, S. G. 199. Courtship period of Adélie, Gentoo, and chinstrap penguins. In: Penguin Biology (Ed. by L. S. Davis & J. T. Darby), pp. 113 127. New York: Academic Press. Trivers, R. L. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man 1871 1971 (Ed. by B. Campbell), pp. 136 179. Chicago: Aldine. Viñuela, J., Amat, J. A. & Ferrer, M. 1995. Nest defence of nesting chinstrap penguins (Pygoscelis antarctica) against intruders. Ethology, 99, 323 331. Weimerskirch, H., Stahl, J. C. & Jouventin, P. 1992. The breeding biology and population dynamics of king penguins Aptenodytes patagonica on the Crozet Islands. Ibis, 134, 17 117. Wiklund, C. G. 1982. Fieldfare (Turdus pilaris) breeding success in relation to colony size, nest position and association with merlins (Falco columbarius). Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology, 11, 165 172. Williams, G. C. 1966. Natural selection, costs of reproduction and a refinement of Lack s principle. American Naturalist, 1, 687 69. Williams, T. D. 1995. The Penguins. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Wittenberger, J. F. & Hunt, G. L., Jr. 1985. The adaptive significance of coloniality in birds. In: Avian Biology (Ed. by D. S. Farner, J. R. King & K. C. Parkes), pp. 1 78. New York: Academic Press.