Visitor Influence on the Behavior of Captive Greater Rheas (Rhea americana, Rheidae Aves)

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Visitor Influence on the Behavior of Captive Greater Rheas (Rhea americana, Rheidae Aves)"

Transcription

1 This article was downloaded by: [Cristiano Azevedo] On: 29 March 2012, At: 12:32 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: Registered office: Mortimer House, Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: Visitor Influence on the Behavior of Captive Greater Rheas (Rhea americana, Rheidae Aves) Cristiano Schetini de Azevedo a, Márcia Fontes Figueiredo Lima b, Vitor Caetano Alves da Silva b, Robert John Young c & Marcos Rodrigues a a Ornithology Laboratory, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Brazil b Department of Biological Sciences, Centro Universitário de Belo, Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil c Conservation, Ecology and Animal Behavior Group, Pontifíca Universidade Católica de Minas Gerais, Brazil Available online: 29 Mar 2012 To cite this article: Cristiano Schetini de Azevedo, Márcia Fontes Figueiredo Lima, Vitor Caetano Alves da Silva, Robert John Young & Marcos Rodrigues (2012): Visitor Influence on the Behavior of Captive Greater Rheas (Rhea americana, Rheidae Aves), Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 15:2, To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

2 Full terms and conditions of use: This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

3 JOURNAL OF APPLIED ANIMAL WELFARE SCIENCE, 15: , 2012 Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: print/ online DOI: / Visitor Influence on the Behavior of Captive Greater Rheas (Rhea americana, Rheidae Aves) Cristiano Schetini de Azevedo, 1 Márcia Fontes Figueiredo Lima, 2 Vitor Caetano Alves da Silva, 2 Robert John Young, 3 and Marcos Rodrigues 1 1 Ornithology Laboratory, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Brazil 2 Department of Biological Sciences, Centro Universitário de Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil 3 Conservation, Ecology and Animal Behavior Group, Pontifíca Universidade Católica de Minas Gerais, Brazil Visitors can affect and reduce the welfare of nonhuman animals. The Belo Horizonte Zoo, Brazil, had a group of greater rheas intended for reintroduction to the wild. Because this group received public visitation, evaluating its effect on the birds behavior and welfare was important. The study conducted 60 hr of behavioral observations: 30 with, and 30 without, visitors in front of the birds enclosure. The study, conducted April December 2009, collected data using scan sampling with instantaneous recording of behavior every minute. The study collected data on public behavior, visitor density, and time spent observing the birds. More than 4,000 persons visited the birds enclosure: 9.86 s average time spent. Public behaviors most expressed were walking-watching-talking and stopped-watchingtalking; visitors or not, greater rheas most expressed behaviors (inversed between treatments) were foraging and walking alert; defecating/urinating and other behaviors differed statistically between treatments. Walking alert was most expressed in the presence of visitors; defecating/urinating and other behaviors were most expressed in their absence. Greater rheas seemed to habituate to visitors. Birds behaviors differed little in visitors presence or absence. Correspondence should be sent to Cristiano Schetini de Azevedo, Laboratório de Ornitologia, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Av.: Antônio Carlos, 6627, Pampulha, Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil. cristianoroxette@yahoo.com 113

4 114 AZEVEDO ET AL. Visitor influence (such as inducing behavioral changes) on the behavior of nonhuman animals in captivity has been widely documented (Davey, 2007; Farrand, 2007; Hosey, 2000; Sekar, Rajagopal, & Archunan, 2008). This visitoreffect influence often results in stress for captive animals and can be measured behaviorally (Birke, 2002; Chamove, Hosey, & Schaetzel, 1988; Sekar et al., 2008) and physiologically (Davis, Schaffner, & Smith, 2005; Montanha, Silva, & Boere, 2009; Rajagopal, Archunan, & Sekar, 2011). Stress is defined as the biological responses of an organism to cope with threats to the organism s homeostasis (Moberg & Mench, 2000). Some studies have shown a link between the increase of people in front of exhibits and a greater expression of aggressive behavior (Hosey, 2000; Sekar et al., 2008). Thus, captive animals intended to be part of conservation programs (reintroduction) need to receive special attention in relation to their exposure to visitation. It is predicted that animals who are not exhibited to visitors will experience less stress than animals exposed to visitors (Burrell & Altman, 2006; Davey, 2006; Glatston, Geilvoet-Soeteman, Hora-Pecek, & Van Hoof, 1984); thus, these animals should be preferred for conservation projects such as reintroduction (McDougall, Réale, Sol, & Reader, 2006). However, many zoos do not have off-exhibit enclosures. On the other hand, for captive management, animals habituated to visitors and caretakers would suffer less stress than would nonhabituated animals (Jezierski, Jaworski, & Górecka, 1999). Some authors even suggested that the daily contact with visitors could function as environmental enrichment, providing sources of novelty in the environment, thus increasing the welfare of the captive animals (Hosey, 2000; Margulis, Royos, & Anderson, 2003). This is, at least theoretically, the worst situation for animals intended to be reintroduced to the wild. They could be attracted to the human presence, increasing human-animal conflicts and increasing their chance of being captured or killed (Hayward et al., 2007). Consequently, zoo visitation needs to be evaluated carefully, depending on the species in question and on the goals of the zoo; thus, conservation versus animal welfare is a conflict faced by zoo managers. Greater rheas (Rhea americana, Rheidae Aves) are the largest South American bird (Del Hoyo, Elliot, & Sargatal, 1992; Sick, 1997); they are flightless, omnivorous birds who occur from Brazil to southern Argentina, habiting grasslands and cerrados (savannah-like vegetation) also in Paraguay, Uruguay, and Bolivia (Davies, 2002). Greater rheas populations are diminishing throughout their distribution due to habitat loss, hunting, crop burnings, and egg losses caused by the use of heavy agricultural machinery (Dani, 1993; Sick, 1997). In Minas Gerais State, southeastern Brazil, greater rheas are classified as vulnerable on the International Union for Conservation of Nature s (2011) red list of species threatened with extinction (Machado, Fonseca, Machado, Aguiar, & Lins, 1998).

5 VISITOR INFLUENCE ON GREATER RHEAS BEHAVIOR 115 Considering this scenario, conservation measures need to be implemented to avoid the greater rheas local extinction. Zoos and other institutions that hold these birds constitute the main sources of individuals for potential reintroduction projects; thus, evaluation of the public s effect on the behavior of captive greater rheas is important for the planning of conservation programs. The evaluation of the public s effect on the behavior of captive greater rheas is important for the planning of conservation programs. This is the case of the Belo Horizonte Zoo (BH Zoo) in southeastern Brazil that intends to reintroduce greater rheas in the near future. If negative effects of visitors presence on the birds behavior or welfare are detected, then new management strategies could be implemented to conserve greater rheas natural behavior and to avoid health problems. The aim of this study was to detect the visitor effect on captive greater rheas by comparing their behavioral expression on days with and without public visitation. It was expected that (a) greater rheas would exhibit more abnormal behaviors, such as pacing, on the days with visitation; (b) larger numbers of visitors would elicit more abnormal behavior; and (c) the intensity of visitation (visitor behavior) would influence greater rheas behavior. The greater rheas behaviors (alert, running, walking alert, defecating/urinating, pacing, eating feces, and drinking water) were considered indicative of stress (Azevedo & Young, 2006; Codenotti, Beninca, & Alvarez, 1995). The behaviors (eating feces, pacing, and drinking water excessively) were considered abnormal due to their lack of presence in conspecifics in the wild or to their high rate of expression during this study s pilot study. In the pilot study, rheas drank water for more than 12% of their daily activity budget. Greater rheas normally drink water for less than 4% of their activity budget (Azevedo, Ferraz, Tinoco, Young, & Rodrigues, 2010; Codenotti et al., 1995). All other behaviors were considered relaxed behaviors as shown in the rheas ethogram (Table 1). Alert and walking alert are behaviors normally expressed during vigilance for predators or during male-male fights for mating (Martella, Renison, & Navarro, 1995); running is expressed during events of predation or during fights between males (Azevedo & Young, 2006; Codenotti et al., 1995). Defecating/urinating is a normal behavior; however, its expression increases during stressful or fear-inducing events (Sanger, Yoshida, Yahyah, & Kitazumi, 2000). MATERIAL AND METHODS Five greater rheas were studied (2 males and 3 females) who were held in the Bird Square of the Belo Horizonte Zoo (BH Zoo), Minas Gerais, Brazil (S 19 ı ; W 44 ı ). The greater rheas exhibit was open to public

6 116 AZEVEDO ET AL. TABLE 1 Ethogram of the Greater Rheas (Rhea americana, Rheidae Aves) at Belo Horizonte Zoo Behavior Description Alert Walking Foraging Inactive Eating Drinking water Running Fighting Preening Threatening Eating feces Pacing Vocalization Mating Incubating eggs Courting Walking alert Play-escape Pecking Dust bathing Defecating/urinating Other behaviors Not visible Rhea stretches neck, elevating head, and moves head from side to side, watching vicinity. Rhea walks through enclosure. Rhea searches for food on the ground or on the vegetation. Rhea stands or sits inactive. Rhea eats the food provided by keeper. Rhea drinks water from the water hole. Rhea runs straight or in zigzags through enclosure. Two males stretch their necks, striking each other with thrusts and bill snapping. Rhea preens feathers with beak. Rhea opens bill and gapes, drawing head back slightly and turning it so as to face the other rhea continually. Rhea eats own or other rheas feces. Rhea walks continually in an eight-shaped route for no apparent reason. Rhea produces a deep-toned two-note call. The female lies flat on the ground as the male grasps the feathers on the back of her neck with his bill; he rests on his legs while copulating. The male incubates the eggs in the nest. The male slowly approaches the female, lowers his neck in a low loop, and walks beside her, sometimes bobbing his head slightly. Rhea walks with an alert posture. Rhea jumps in the air and then run in zigzags as soon as he or she touches the ground, for no apparent reason. Rhea pecks objects in the ground or the enclosure s fence. Rhea extends neck and picks up some soil or dust with bill, throwing the dust onto own wings and body. Rhea defecates or urinates. Behaviors not previously recorded. Birds not visible to the observer. visitation every week, Tuesday Sunday, 9:00 a.m. 5:00 p.m. Birds were housed in a wire-fenced enclosure 28 m in length and 16 m wide. The front of the enclosure was the visitors viewing area (28 m length); a female cassowary (Casuarius casuarius) and two ostriches (Struthio camelus) were housed in neighboring enclosures. Rheas were fed twice a day with a mixture of ratite ration (Socil ; 1.2 kg in total) and vegetables (carrot and cabbage; 1.3 kg in total). Water was provided ad libitum in a water hole. The enclosure was cleaned three times a week (Tuesdays, Thursdays, and Saturdays) but never during the data-collection period. Birds ages ranged from 4 to 8 years ( years) and the birds had been exposed to the public between ages 3 and 7 years. Birds younger than 1 year were not exposed ( years).

7 VISITOR INFLUENCE ON GREATER RHEAS BEHAVIOR 117 The study was divided into two treatments: (a) the presence of visitors in front of the greater rheas enclosure and (b) the absence of visitors in front of the greater rheas enclosure. Data collection in the absence of the public was conducted only on Mondays, when the zoo was closed to visitation. In addition, no keeper was allowed to enter or pass in front of the rheas enclosure during data collection. Data collection in the presence of the public was conducted on Tuesdays and weekends, days when the BH Zoo receives most of its visitors; the flow of the public in front of the greater rheas enclosure was continuous. Thirty hours of behavioral data were collected in each treatment. All data were collected using scan sampling with instantaneous recording of behaviors every minute (Altmann, 1974). All behavioral data-collection sessions were conducted between 1500h and 1600h daily, April December This period was chosen because pilot observations had shown rheas to be most active at this time of day. An ethogram for greater rheas (Table 1) was constructed. The ethogram was based on scientific literature (Brandt & Neto, 1999; Codenotti et al., 1995) and on 12 hr of ad libitum pilot observations (Martin & Bateson, 2007). Stress-related activities were inferred only on behavioral changes and were not measured physiologically. Data on behavior of the public were also collected in the same time period by a second observer. The study used scan-sampling data collection for more than two visitors, and behaviors of all visitors in the groups were recorded. Focal sampling was used (when only one visitor was present), with instantaneous recording of behaviors every minute (Altmann, 1974). An ethogram of the visitors behavior (Table 2) was based on 10 hr of ad libitum pilot observations (Martin & Bateson, 2007). Data concerning visitor number (number of visitors per hour in front of the greater rheas enclosure) and time spent in front of the greater rheas enclosure (period each visitor or group of visitors stayed in front of the greater rheas enclosure) were also collected by a third observer. Tests of interobserver reliability were conducted during the 1 month of pilot observations. Data on greater rheas behavior was quantified (total number of each behavior recordings per day) and tested with an Anderson-Darling test to determine if they met the requirements for parametric statistics, which they did not; therefore, nonparametric statistical tests were used throughout. The Mann-Whitney U test was used to compare the mean number of behavioral recordings in both presence and absence of the visitors in front of the greater rheas enclosure. Data on the visitors behavior was quantified; focal and scan samplings were summed and the total number of each behavior recordings per day analyzed. Results of the visitor-behavior analysis are presented as percentages. Spearman s rank correlations between (a) behaviors expressed by the greater rheas and by the visitors and between (b) behaviors expressed by the greater

8 118 AZEVEDO ET AL. TABLE 2 Ethogram of the Visitors Behavior at the Greater Rhea Exhibit at Belo Horizonte Zoo Behavior Reading label Walking-watching-quietly Walking-watching-talking Walking-watching-shouting Walking-not-watching-quietly Walking-not-watching-talking Walking-not-watching-shouting Running Stopped-watching-quietly Stopped-watching-talking Stopped-watching-shouting Stopped-not-watching-quietly Stopped-not-watching-talking Stopped-not-watching-shouting Interacting with the rheas Other behaviors Not visible Description Visitor reads the label of the rhea s exhibit. Visitor walks quietly through the visitor s viewing area when watching the rheas. Visitor walks talking through the visitor s viewing area when watching the rheas. Visitor walks shouting through the visitor s viewing area when watching the rheas. Visitor walks quietly through the visitor s viewing area but not watching the rheas. Visitor walks talking through the visitor s viewing area but not watching the rheas. Visitor walks shouting through the visitor s viewing area but not watching the rheas. Visitor runs through the visitor s viewing area of the greater rheas exhibit. Visitor stops in the visitor s viewing area and quietly watches the rheas. Visitor stops in the visitor s viewing area and talks while watching the rheas. Visitor stops in the visitor s viewing area and shouts while watching the rheas. Visitor stops quietly in the visitor s viewing area but not watching the rheas. Visitor stops in the visitor s viewing area and talks but not watching the rheas. Visitor stops in the visitor s viewing area and shouts but not watching the rheas. Visitor interacts with the rheas, throwing food to the birds or calling the birds to approach him or her. Any other behavior expressed that is not in this ethogram. Birds not visible to the observer. rheas and the number of visitors were undertaken. For all statistical analyses, the confidence level was 95% ( D 0.05; Zar, 1999). RESULTS The number of the public who visited the greater rheas enclosure during the study period was 4,182, with a mean of 139.4% ( 15.3) per hour. The minimum number of visitors in front of the greater rheas enclosure at the same time

9 VISITOR INFLUENCE ON GREATER RHEAS BEHAVIOR 119 was 1; the largest group of visitors had 103 individuals. The mean time spent by the visitors in front of the greater rheas enclosure was 9.86 ( 1.49) s, the minimum time spent by the visitors in front of the greater rheas enclosure was 9 s, and the maximum time was 319 s. The percentages of behaviors expressed by the visitors are shown in Figure 1. The most performed visitor behaviors were as follows: stopped-watching-talking (40.08%) and walking-watchingtalking (30.25%). The least exhibited behaviors were walking-watching-shouting (0.17%) and interacting-with-the-birds (0.33%); walking-not-watching-shouting behavior was not performed by the visitors during the study. The most expressed behaviors by the greater rheas in the presence of the public were foraging (19.12%) followed by walking (18.62%); the least exhibited behaviors by the greater rheas in the presence of the public were play-escape (0.03%) and running (0.04%). The most expressed behaviors by the greater rheas without the presence of the visitors were walking (21.71%) and foraging (19.49%); the least exhibited behaviors by the greater rheas without the presence of the visitors were play-escape and intimidating, both with only three records (0.03% each). Only walking alert, defecating/urinating, and other behaviors differed statistically between the treatments (Table 1). Walking alert was expressed more often in the presence of visitors, whereas defecating/urinating and other behaviors were more often expressed in the absence of the visitors (Table 3). A negative significant correlation was found between the following behaviors: alert and walking-watching-talking (r s D 0.64, N D 30, p <.01); alert and FIGURE 1 Percentage of visitor behaviors in front of the greater rheas enclosure at Belo Horizonte Zoo. RL D reading label; WWQ D walking-watching-quietly; WWT D walking-watching-talking; WWS D walking-watching-shouting; WNWQ D walkingnot-watching-quietly; WNWT D walking-not-watching-talking; WNWS D walking-notwatching-shouting; RUN D running; SWQ D stopped-watching-quietly; SWT D stoppedwatching-talking; SWS D stopped-watching-shouting; INT D interacting with the rheas; OTH D other behaviors; NV D not visible.

10 120 AZEVEDO ET AL. TABLE 3 Behaviors (Daily Mean Number of Observations) Expressed by the Greater Rheas of BH Zoo in the Presence and Absence of Visitors in Front of Their Enclosure (Mean Standard Error, DF D 1, N D 30 in All Cases) and Mann-Whitney U Test Results Behavior With Public Without Public Mann- Whitney p Value Alert Walking Foraging Inactive Eating Drinking water Running Fighting Preening Threatening Eating feces Pacing Vocalization Mating Incubating eggs Courting Walking alert a Play escape Pecking Dust bathing Defecating/Urinating a Other behaviors <.01 a Not visible a Behaviors that differed statistically between treatments. running (public; r s D 0.37, N D 30, p <.05); alert and stopped-watchingtalking (r s D 0.47, N D 30, p <.01); preening; stopped-watching-talking (r s D 0.49, N D 30, p <.01); pacing; and other behaviors (public; r s D 0.58, N D 30, p <.01). A positive correlation was also found between foraging and stopped-watching-quietly behaviors (r s D 0.54, N D 30, p <.01). The first behaviors in the correlation referred to greater rheas behaviors; second behaviors in the correlation referred to the public s behaviors). A negative, significant correlation was found between the number of visitors in front of the exhibit and the following greater rheas behaviors: alert, pacing, and not visible (r s D 0.59, N D 30, p <.01, r s D 0.38, N D 30, p <.05, r s D 0.42, N D 30, p <.05).

11 VISITOR INFLUENCE ON GREATER RHEAS BEHAVIOR 121 DISCUSSION Greater rheas seemed habituated to visitor presence; the majority of their behaviors did not differ in either the presence or absence of visitors in front of their enclosure. Habituation is good for greater rheas captive management because it probably, reduces stress (Owen, Swaisgood, Czekala, Steinman, & Lindburg, 2004; Waitt, Buchanan-Smith, & Morris, 2002). For conservation programs, however, habituation should be avoided. After release, rheas might approach humans; such approaches could cause problems for the birds (Kloppers, St. Clair, & Hurd, 2005; Lamarque et al., 2008). Only the following behaviors differed between treatments: walking alert, defecating/urinating, and other behaviors. Walking alert was more often expressed when the visitors were present in front of the greater rheas enclosure. This could be vigilance (antipredator) response; that is, with the presence of people, greater rheas became more stressed. Greater rheas increased their vigilance when presented with predators (stuffed jaguar and a dog) in an antipredator training study conducted by Azevedo and Young (2006). Stress due to visitors presence has been observed for primates in many studies (Chamove et al., 1988; Wells, 2005; Wood, 1998). Studies include the following: Indian leopards (Panthera pardus; Mallapur & Chellam, 2002), black rhinos (Diceros bicornis; Carlstead & Brown, 2005), birds (Bennet & Zuelke, 1999), and Indian gaurs (Bos gaurus; Sekar et al., 2008). Three factors influence vigilance in greater rheas in the wild: size of the group, sex of the bird, and time of year. Solitary birds exhibit more vigilance behavior than do groups of 2 or more birds (Martella et al., 1995; Reboreda & Fernandez, 1997); males exhibit more vigilance behaviors than do females (Reboreda & Fernandez, 1997); and, during breeding season, rheas become more vigilant (Azevedo et al., 2010). These variables did not seem to have influenced the results found in this study. All data in this study were recorded during greater rheas breeding season. Of 5 birds studied (Reboreda & Fernandez, 1997), none showed differences in vigilance behaviors between groups of 2 to 26; further, the sex ratio of the group was nearly 1:1. Defecating/urinating was most exhibited when the public was absent from the visitor area; when frequently expressed, this behavior may be associated with fear (Candland & Campbell, 1962; Munksgaard, de Passile, Rushen, Thodberg, & Jensen, 1997; Vargas, Marques, & Schenberg, 2000). Such association was not the case in this study; the behavior occupied only 0.2% ( ) of the greater rheas activity budget. Thus, the recording of this behavior was disproportionately higher in the absence of visitors, probably due to chance. Other behaviors such as head scratching, stretching, and building nests were also more often expressed when the public was absent from the visitor area. These behaviors could indicate a relaxed behavioral state; because without the

12 122 AZEVEDO ET AL. presence of the public, greater rheas expressed significantly more of these relaxed behaviors, their low occurrence in the presence of visitors could be interpreted as a low source of stress to the birds. It is important to emphasize that other behaviors were expressed at very low levels (less than 1% of the activity-time budget; 0.2% in the presence and 0.7% in the absence of visitors). None of the abnormal behaviors differed statistically between treatments. Most of the behaviors expressed by the visitors were not considered threats to the welfare of the greater rheas. All the correlations found between visitor and rhea behavior supported this statement. Shouting and rhea-visitor interactions were rare, and the time spent by the visitors in front of the greater rheas enclosure was minimal (9.8 s on average). Public flux was high; this could act as a source of novelty for the birds, even functioning as environmental enrichment (Morris, 1964; Pazol & Bloomsmith, 1993). The negative correlations found between the alert, pacing, and not visible behaviors support this hypothesis. In this study, audience activities and noisiness, the most common variables inferred as sources of animal stress (Birke, 2002; Hosey & Druck, 1987; Mitchell, Herring, & Obradovich, 1992), did not appear to show great negative effects on greater rheas welfare. The behaviors expressed by the rheas were also influenced by a number of other variables not measured in this study; thus, it is important to remember that generalizations of these results should be made with care. For example, contact with zookeepers and the regular maintenance of the enclosure (cleaning days) had been reduced due to the prohibition of such activities during the periods of data collection. In addition, other factors could have influenced the results (Mitchell & Hosey, 2005): frequency of camera flashes, objects (such as umbrellas) carried by the public, smoking, sex and age of the visitors, and period of data collection. Behavioral data collection at different times of the day could have shown differences, related to the effect of the public, in the activity budget of the rheas. For example, had rheas rested in the morning when no visitors were present but did not rest when visitors were present, this would be a better indicator of visitor-induced stress. CONCLUSION The results found in this study showed that the greater rheas of BH Zoo appeared habituated to the presence of the public. There were, however, animal welfare implications. The greater rheas habituation to the public indicates visitors probably were not a significant source of stimuli causing animal welfare problems (stress). To avoid changes in this scenario, it is important that the zoo s staff provide educational support to the public visiting the greater rhea s enclosure, namely, to avoid shouting or performing behaviors that might be

13 VISITOR INFLUENCE ON GREATER RHEAS BEHAVIOR 123 stress-inducing for the birds. The greater rheas in this study did not show signs of stress problems; however, density and intensity of visitors have been shown to affect the behavior of captive animals (Hosey & Druck, 1987; Sellinger & Ha, 2005). For animals intended for reintroduction to the wild, the results found in this study are worrying. Habituated animals have a greater probability of dying after release than nonhabituated animals (Kasereka, Muhigwa, Shalukoma, & Kahekwa, 2006). On the other hand, the maintenance of nonhabituated animals in the zoo could generate welfare problems due to acute or chronic stress resulting from daily contact with humans (Moberg & Mench, 2000). ACKNOWLEDGMENTS MR thanks the Fundação O Boticário de Proteção à Natureza for supporting the Laboratório de Ornitologia of Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais. Robert John Young and Marcos Rodrigues are supported by Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa de Minas Gerais (Pesquisador Mineiro) and Conselho Nacional de Pesquisa (Bolsa de Produtividade). We thank Angela Faggioli and Carlyle Mendes Coelho for permission to use the Belo Horizonte Zoo s facilities and birds and for their invaluable suggestions on this article. REFERENCES Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behavior, 49, Azevedo, C. S., Ferraz, J. B., Tinoco, H. P., Young, R. J., & Rodrigues, M. (2010). Time-activity budget of greater rheas (Rhea americana, Aves) on a human-disturbed area: The role of habitat, time of the day, season and group size. Acta Ethologica, 13, Azevedo, C. S., & Young, R. J. (2006). Behavioral responses of captive-born greater rheas (Rhea americana) Linnaeus (Rheiformes, Rheidae) submitted to antipredator training. Revista Brasileira de Zoologia, 23, Bennet, K. A., & Zuelke, E. F. (1999). The effects of recreation on birds: A literature review. Wilmington, DE: Department of Natural Resources and Environmental Control. Birke, L. (2002). Effects of browse, human visitors and noise on the behavior of captive orangutans. Animal Welfare, 11, Brandt, L. F. S., & Neto, A. S. (1999). Introdução e Monitoramento de Rhea americana na EPDA Galheiro (Perdizes, MG) [Introduction and monitoring of Rhea americana at EPDA Galheiro (Perdizes, MG)]. Belo Horizonte, Brazil: CEMIG. Burrell, A. M., & Altman, J. D. (2006). The effect of captive environment on activity of captive cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 9, Candland, D. K., & Campbell, B. A. (1962). Development of fear in the rat as measured by behavior in the open field. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, Carlstead, K., & Brown, J. L. (2005). Relationships between patterns of fecal corticoid excretion and behavior, reproduction, and environmental factors in captive black (Diceros bicornis) and white (Ceratotherium simum) rhinoceros. Zoo Biology, 24,

14 124 AZEVEDO ET AL. Chamove, A. S., Hosey, G. R., & Schaetzel, P. (1988). Visitors excite primates in zoos. Zoo Biology, 7, Codenotti, T. L., Beninca, D., & Alvarez, F. (1995). Etograma y relacion de la conducta con el habitat y con la edad en el ñandú [Ethogram and the relation of the behavior of greater rheas with habitat and age]. Doñana, Acta Vertebrata, 22(12), Dani, S. (1993). A Ema Rhea americana: Biologia, manejo e conservação [The greater Rhea americana: Biology, management, and conservation]. Belo Horizonte, Brazil: Fundação Acangaú. Davey, G. (2006). Visitor behavior in zoos: A review. Anthrozoos, 19, Davey, G. (2007). Visitors effects on the welfare of animals in the zoo: A review. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 10, Davies, S. J. J. F. (2002). Ratites and tinamous. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Davis, N., Schaffner, C. M., & Smith, T. E. (2005). Evidence that zoo visitors influence HPA activity in spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyii rufiventris). Applied Animal Behavior Science, 90, Del Hoyo, J., Elliot, A., & Sargatal, J. A. (1992). Handbook of the birds of the world (Vol. 1). Barcelona, Spain: Lynx Editions. Farrand, A. (2007). The effect of zoo visitors on the behavior and welfare of zoo mammals (Doctoral thesis, University of Stirling, Edinburgh, Scotland). Glatston, A. R., Geilvoet-Soeteman, E., Hora-Pecek, E., & Van Hoof, J. A. R. A. M. (1984). The influence of the zoo environment on social behavior of groups of cotton-topped tamarins, Saguinus aedipus aedipus. Zoo Biology, 3, Hayward, M. W., Adendorff, J., O Brien, J., Sholto-Douglas, A., Bissett, C., Moolman, L. C., : : : Kerley, G. I. H. (2007). Practical considerations for the reintroduction of large, terrestrial, mammalian predators based on reintroductions to South Africa s Eastern Cape Province. The Open Conservation Biology Journal, 1, Hosey, G. R. (2000). Zoo animals and their human audiences: What is the visitor effect? Animal Welfare, 9, Hosey, G. R., & Druck, P. L. (1987). The influence of zoo visitors on the behavior of captive primates. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 18, International Union for Conservation of Nature. (2011). IUCN Red List of Threatened Species (Version ). Retrieved from Jezierski, T., Jaworski, Z., & Górecka, A. (1999). Effects of handling on behaviour and heart rate in konik horses: Comparison of stable and forest reared young stock. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 62, Kasereka, B., Muhigwa, J. B., Shalukoma, C., & Kahekwa, J. M. (2006). Vulnerability of habituated Grauer s gorilla to poaching in the Kahuzi-Biega National Park, DRC. African Study Monographs, 27, Kloppers, E. L., St. Clair, C. C., & Hurd, T. E. (2005). Predator-resembling aversive conditioning for managing habituated wildlife. Ecology and Society, 10, 31. Lamarque, F., Anderson, J., Chardonnet, P., Fergusson, R., Lagrange, M., Osei-Owusu, Y., : : : Tabi Tako-Eta, P. (2008). Human-wildlife conflict in Africa: An overview of causes, consequences and management strategies. Rome, Italy: International Foundation for the Conservation of Wildlife, Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations. Machado, A. B. M., Fonseca, G. A. B., Machado, R. B., Aguiar, L. M. S., & Lins, L. V. (1998). Livro vermelho das espécies ameaçadas de extinção da fauna de Minas Gerais [The red list of animal threatened species of Minas Gerais]. Belo Horizonte, Brazil: Fundação Biodiversitas. Mallapur, A., & Chellam, R. (2002). Environmental influences on stereotypy and the activity budget of Indian leopards (Panthera pardus) in four zoos in Southern India. Zoo Biology, 21, Margulis, S. W., Royos, C., & Anderson, M. (2003). Effect of felid activity on zoo visitor interest. Zoo Biology, 22, Martella, M. B., Renison, D., & Navarro, J. T. (1995). Vigilance in the greater rhea: Effects of vegetation height and group size. Journal of Field Ornithology, 66,

15 VISITOR INFLUENCE ON GREATER RHEAS BEHAVIOR 125 Martin, P., & Bateson, P. (2007). Measuring behavior: An introductory guide (3rd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. McDougall, P. T., Réale, D., Sol, D., & Reader, S. M. (2006). Wildlife conservation and animal temperament: Causes and consequences of evolutionary change for captive, reintroduced, and wild populations. Animal Conservation, 9, Mitchell, G., Herring, F., & Obradovich, S. (1992). Like threaten like in mangabeys and people? Anthrozoos, 5, Mitchell, H., & Hosey, G Zoo research guidelines: Studies on the effects of human visitors on zoo animal behavior. London, UK: British and Irish Association of Zoos and Aquariums. Moberg, G. P., & Mench, J. A. (2000). The biology of animal stress: Basic principles and implications for animal welfare. Wallingford, UK: CABI. Montanha, J. C., Silva, S. L., & Boere, V. (2009). Comparison of salivary cortisol concentrations in jaguar kept in captivity with differences in exposure to the public. Ciência Rural, 39, Morris, D. (1964). The response of animals to a restricted environment. Symposium of the Zoological Society of London, 13, Munksgaard, L., de Passile, A. M., Rushen, J., Thodberg, K., & Jensen, M. B. (1997). Discrimination of people by dairy cows based on handling. Journal of Dairy Science, 80, Owen, M. A., Swaisgood, R. R., Czekala, N. M., Steinman, K., & Lindburg, D. G. (2004). Monitoring stress in captive giant pandas (Ailuropoda melanoleuca): Behavioral and hormonal responses to ambient noise. Zoo Biology, 23, Pazol, K. A., & Bloomsmith, M. A. (1993). The development of stereotyped body rocking in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) reared in a variety of nursery settings. Animal Welfare, 2, Rajagopal, T., Archunan, G., & Sekar, M. (2011). Impact of zoo visitors on the fecal cortisol levels and behavior of an endangered species: Indian blackbuck (Antelope cervicapra L.). Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 14, Reboreda, J. C., & Fernandez, G. J. (1997). Sexual, seasonal and group size differences in the allocation of time between vigilance and feeding in the greater rhea (Rhea americana). Ethology, 103, Sanger, G. J., Yoshida, M., Yahyah, M., & Kitazumi, K Increased defecation during stress or after 5-hydroxytryptophan: Selective inhibition by the 5-HT 4 receptor antagonist, 5B British Journal of Pharmacology, 130, Sekar, M., Rajagopal, T., & Archunan, G. (2008). Influence of zoo visitor presence on the behavior of captive Imdian gaur (Bos gaurus gaurus) in a zoological park. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 11, Sellinger, R. L., & Ha, J. C. (2005). The effects of visitor density and intensity on the behavior of two captive jaguars (Panthera onca). Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 8, Sick, H. (1997). Ornitologia Brasileira [Brazilian ornithology]. Rio de Janeiro, Brazil: Editora Nova Fronteira. Vargas, L. C., Marques, T. A., & Schenberg, L. C. (2000). Micturition and defensive behaviors are controlled by distinct neural networks within the dorsal periaqueductal gray and deep gray layers of the superior colliculus of the rat. Neuroscience Letters, 280, Waitt, C., Buchanan-Smith, H., & Morris, K. (2002). The effects of caretaker-primate relationships on primates on the laboratory. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 5, Wells, D. L. (2005). A note on the influence of visitors on the behavior and welfare of zoo-housed gorillas. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 93, Wood, W. (1998). Interactions among environmental enrichment, viewing crowds, and zoo chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Zoo Biology, 17, Young, R. J. (2003). Environmental enrichment for captive animals. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Zar, J. H. (1999). Biostatistical analysis. Newark, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Published online: 24 Sep 2008.

Published online: 24 Sep 2008. This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 07:48 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Distressed Animal Behaviors and Some Recommendations for Improvements at the Kuala Lumpur Zoo, Malaysia Amber Haque Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Distressed Animal Behaviors and Some Recommendations for Improvements at the Kuala Lumpur Zoo, Malaysia Amber Haque Published online: 04 Jun 2010. This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 06:55 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

This is the peer reviewed version of this article

This is the peer reviewed version of this article Farrand A, Hosey G & Buchanan-Smith HM (2014) The visitor effect in petting zoo-housed animals: Aversive or enriching?, Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 151, pp. 117-127. This is the peer reviewed version

More information

The Value of Data Gary Patronek & Stephen Zawistowski Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

The Value of Data Gary Patronek & Stephen Zawistowski Published online: 04 Jun 2010. This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 08 June 2015, At: 09:24 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Hsin-Yi Weng a & Lynette A. Hart b a Department of Pathobiology, College of Veterinary

Hsin-Yi Weng a & Lynette A. Hart b a Department of Pathobiology, College of Veterinary This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 10:20 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Visitor effects on the behavior of drills (Mandrillus leucophaeus) and petting zoo animals at Parken Zoo. Lisa Lundin

Visitor effects on the behavior of drills (Mandrillus leucophaeus) and petting zoo animals at Parken Zoo. Lisa Lundin Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology Master Thesis Visitor effects on the behavior of drills (Mandrillus leucophaeus) and petting zoo animals at Parken Zoo Lisa Lundin LiTH-IFM- Ex-- 13/2769 --SE

More information

The Use of Cage Enrichment to Reduce Male Mouse Aggression Neil Ambrose & David B. Morton Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

The Use of Cage Enrichment to Reduce Male Mouse Aggression Neil Ambrose & David B. Morton Published online: 04 Jun 2010. This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 08 June 2015, At: 08:36 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Departments, Iowa State University, Ames b Department of Population Medicine, Ontario Veterinary College, University of Guelph,

Departments, Iowa State University, Ames b Department of Population Medicine, Ontario Veterinary College, University of Guelph, This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 07:54 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Meredith J. Bashaw a, Angela S. Kelling b, Mollie A. Bloomsmith b & Terry L. Maple b a TECHlab, Zoo Atlanta, and Center for

Meredith J. Bashaw a, Angela S. Kelling b, Mollie A. Bloomsmith b & Terry L. Maple b a TECHlab, Zoo Atlanta, and Center for This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 07:05 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Gruiformes Family: Cariamidae Scientific Name: Cariama cristata Common Name: Red-legged seriema

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Gruiformes Family: Cariamidae Scientific Name: Cariama cristata Common Name: Red-legged seriema Order: Gruiformes Family: Cariamidae Scientific Name: Cariama cristata Common Name: Red-legged seriema AZA Management: Green Yellow Red None Photo (Male): Red-legged seriemas are identical in plumage although

More information

Unit 3 Sustainability and interdependence Sub Topic 3.4: Animal welfare

Unit 3 Sustainability and interdependence Sub Topic 3.4: Animal welfare Unit 3 Sustainability and interdependence Sub Topic 3.4: Animal welfare Page 1 of 12 On completion of this topic I will be able to: Describe the costs, benefits and ethics of providing different levels

More information

The Effect of Zoo Visitors on the Behaviour and Welfare of Zoo Mammals. Alexandra Farrand

The Effect of Zoo Visitors on the Behaviour and Welfare of Zoo Mammals. Alexandra Farrand The Effect of Zoo Visitors on the Behaviour and Welfare of Zoo Mammals Alexandra Farrand Department of Psychology University of Stirling Thesis submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree

More information

To cite this article: Emily Weiss & Sandra Wilson (2003) The Use of Classical and

To cite this article: Emily Weiss & Sandra Wilson (2003) The Use of Classical and This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 08 June 2015, At: 09:40 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Effects of construction noise on behaviour of and exhibit use by Snow leopards Uncia uncia at Basel zoo

Effects of construction noise on behaviour of and exhibit use by Snow leopards Uncia uncia at Basel zoo BASEL ZOO: EFFECTS OF NOISE ON BEHAVIOUR AND EXHIBIT USE BY SNOW LEOPARDS 199 Int. Zoo Yb. (2008) 42: 199 205 DOI:10.1111/j.1748-1090.2007.00040.x Effects of construction noise on behaviour of and exhibit

More information

A preliminary model of human animal relationships in the zoo

A preliminary model of human animal relationships in the zoo Applied Animal Behaviour Science 109 (2008) 105 127 www.elsevier.com/locate/applanim Review A preliminary model of human animal relationships in the zoo Geoff Hosey Psychology & Life Sciences, University

More information

MATING BEHAVIOR OF THE MALE GREATER RHEA

MATING BEHAVIOR OF THE MALE GREATER RHEA MATING BEHAVIOR OF THE MALE GREATER RHEA Author(s) :THAÏS L. CODENOTTI and FERNANDO ALVAREZ Source: The Wilson Bulletin, 113(1):85-89. 2001. Published By: The Wilson Ornithological Society DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1676/0043-5643(2001)113[0085:mbotmg]2.0.co;2

More information

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl) Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl) Family: Strigidae (Typical Owls) Order: Strigiformes (Owls) Class: Aves (Birds) Fig. 1. Mottled owl, Ciccaba virgata. [http://www.owling.com/mottled13.htm, downloaded 12 November

More information

Can signs with salient eyes help mitigate visitor effects on the behaviour of zoo-housed Sulawesi crested macaques (Macaca nigra)?

Can signs with salient eyes help mitigate visitor effects on the behaviour of zoo-housed Sulawesi crested macaques (Macaca nigra)? Can signs with salient eyes help mitigate visitor effects on the behaviour of zoo-housed Sulawesi crested macaques (Macaca nigra)? Alice Dancer, ZSL & RVC and Dr Charlotte Burn, RVC Sulawesi crested macaque

More information

Comparison of Several Types of Enrichment for Captive Felids

Comparison of Several Types of Enrichment for Captive Felids Zoo Biology 26:371 381 (2007) RESEARCH ARTICLE Comparison of Several Types of Enrichment for Captive Felids Amy L. Skibiel, 1 Heather S. Trevino, 1 and Ken Naugher 2 1 Department of Biological Sciences,

More information

Kori Bustard Husbandry. Sara Hallager, Biologist, Smithsonian National Zoological Park

Kori Bustard Husbandry. Sara Hallager, Biologist, Smithsonian National Zoological Park Kori Bustard Husbandry Sara Hallager, Biologist, Smithsonian National Zoological Park Ardeotis kori 2 subspecies [?] Africa s largest flying bird Captive males: 12-19kg Seasonal weight gain up to 4kg Captive

More information

Carin Wittnich a b & Michael Belanger b a Canadian Veterinary Medical Association, Ottawa,

Carin Wittnich a b & Michael Belanger b a Canadian Veterinary Medical Association, Ottawa, This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 07:32 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Mental stim ulation it s not just for dogs!! By Danielle Middleton- Beck BSc hons, PGDip CABC

Mental stim ulation it s not just for dogs!! By Danielle Middleton- Beck BSc hons, PGDip CABC Milo, Congo African Grey by Elaine Henley Mental stim ulation it s not just for dogs!! By Danielle Middleton- Beck BSc hons, PGDip CABC Dexter, Green Iguana by Danielle Middleton-Beck Exotic pets include

More information

Health and Welfare of Resreach Animals. Richard E. Brown Psychology Department Dalhousie University Halifax, Nova Scotia Canada B3H 4J1

Health and Welfare of Resreach Animals. Richard E. Brown Psychology Department Dalhousie University Halifax, Nova Scotia Canada B3H 4J1 Health and Welfare of Resreach Animals Richard E. Brown Psychology Department Dalhousie University Halifax, Nova Scotia Canada B3H 4J1 What is Animal Welfare? Concern for the well-being of research animals

More information

Behaviour of cats and dogs

Behaviour of cats and dogs Behaviour of cats and dogs Unlike cats, dogs are social animals living in packs. Dogs normally live in a group with a well developed social hierarchy and communicate by sight, sound, smell and use of body

More information

> BACK TO CONTENTS PAGE

> BACK TO CONTENTS PAGE Human interaction: previously pursued for their feathers; nowadays farmed for meat. In the wild they will attack if threatened (treacherous kick); passive in captive environments. If raised, they may display

More information

Talking about zoos, animal well-being and education with Jon Coe

Talking about zoos, animal well-being and education with Jon Coe Talking about zoos, animal well-being and education with Jon Coe Philadelphia Zoo por Teresa Sauquet @TeresaSauquet, vocal de Conservación y Bienestar de la @APEspain conservacion@apespain.org It was 1966,

More information

Ratite Standards and Guidelines

Ratite Standards and Guidelines Exhibited Animals - Ratite and Australian Animal Welfare and Exhibited Animals - Ratite and December 2011 Page 1 of 18 Exhibited Animals - Ratite and Introduction Purpose The principal purpose of this

More information

Social and Reproductive Behaviours in the Cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus) in A Captive Population

Social and Reproductive Behaviours in the Cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus) in A Captive Population Social and Reproductive Behaviours in the Cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus) in A Captive Population Rebecca Bradford-Wright B.A. Hons A THESIS SUBMITTED FOR THE DEGREE DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY School of Biological

More information

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis I. P. JOHNSON and R. M. SIBLY Fourteen individually marked pairs o f Canada Geese were observedfrom January to April on their feeding grounds

More information

Zoo Crew. A SmithSoniAn S national Zoo FAmily EduCAtion GuidE

Zoo Crew. A SmithSoniAn S national Zoo FAmily EduCAtion GuidE Zoo Crew A SmithSoniAn S national Zoo FAmily EduCAtion GuidE join the Zoo CrEw! Are you ready to become a wildlife conservationist and part of the Zoo Crew? During your visit to the Smithsonian s National

More information

Stereotypic pacing in Siberian tiger (Panthera tigris altaica) and the effect of interspecies presence: A case study

Stereotypic pacing in Siberian tiger (Panthera tigris altaica) and the effect of interspecies presence: A case study Stereotypic pacing in Siberian tiger (Panthera tigris altaica) and the effect of interspecies presence: A case study Table of Contents List of Figures... 2 List of Tables... 3 Abstract... 4 1.0 Introduction...

More information

Behavior of Captive Humboldt Penguin (Spheniscus humboldti) Chicks in Response to Environmental Enrichment

Behavior of Captive Humboldt Penguin (Spheniscus humboldti) Chicks in Response to Environmental Enrichment SUNY College of Environmental Science and Forestry Digital Commons @ ESF Honors Theses 2015 Behavior of Captive Humboldt Penguin (Spheniscus humboldti) Chicks in Response to Environmental Enrichment Emma

More information

Financial support: INPA/MCT, Manaus Energia S.A., ReBio Uatumã and Fundação O Boticário de Proteção à Natureza.

Financial support: INPA/MCT, Manaus Energia S.A., ReBio Uatumã and Fundação O Boticário de Proteção à Natureza. SOCIAL ORGANIZATION, BEHAVIOR AND DIET OF GIANT OTTERS (Pteronura brasiliensis) IN THE RESERVOIR OF BALBINA HYDROELECTRIC POWER STATION, AMAZONAS, BRAZIL. TEAM: Dr. Fernando Rosas (coordinator)/inpa. BSc.

More information

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

To link to this article:  PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 08 June 2015, At: 10:22 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Laura M. Dixon a & Ian J. H. Duncan a a Department of Animal and Poultry Science,

Laura M. Dixon a & Ian J. H. Duncan a a Department of Animal and Poultry Science, This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 08:29 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

To link to this article:   PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 06:54 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Long-Term Outcome After Treatment of Feline Inappropriate Elimination Amy R. Marder & Joan M. Engel Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Long-Term Outcome After Treatment of Feline Inappropriate Elimination Amy R. Marder & Joan M. Engel Published online: 04 Jun 2010. This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 08 June 2015, At: 09:33 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

INTRODUCTION & MEASURING ANIMAL BEHAVIOR

INTRODUCTION & MEASURING ANIMAL BEHAVIOR INTRODUCTION & MEASURING ANIMAL BEHAVIOR Photo courtesy: USDA What is behavior? Aggregate of responses to internal and external stimuli - Dictionary.com The action, reaction, or functioning of a system,

More information

FRENZ. World Leading Poultry Layer Standard

FRENZ. World Leading Poultry Layer Standard Celebrating New Zealand F years ree Ranging pasture far med As Nature Intended FRENZ World Leading Poultry Layer Standard Celebrating New Zealand F years ree Ranging pasture far med As Nature Intended

More information

Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey

Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring - 2011 Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey October 2011 1 Cover photograph: Egyptian vulture landing in Beypazarı dump site, photographed

More information

Refinement Issues in Animal Research. Joanne Zurlo, PhD Institute for Laboratory Animal Research National Academy of Sciences

Refinement Issues in Animal Research. Joanne Zurlo, PhD Institute for Laboratory Animal Research National Academy of Sciences This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike License. Your use of this material constitutes acceptance of that license and the conditions of use of materials on this

More information

Breeding and Managing Pheasants

Breeding and Managing Pheasants The World Pheasant Association Breeding and Managing Pheasants John Corder ISBN No: 978 0 906864 16 6 Copyright 2011 World Pheasant Association Published by the World Pheasant Association, Newcastle upon

More information

Role of Temperature and Shade Coverage on Behavior and Habitat Use of Captive African Lions, Snow Leopards, and Cougars

Role of Temperature and Shade Coverage on Behavior and Habitat Use of Captive African Lions, Snow Leopards, and Cougars Xavier Journal of Undergraduate Research Volume 4 Article 7 2016 Role of Temperature and Shade Coverage on Behavior and Habitat Use of Captive African Lions, Snow Leopards, and Cougars Caitlin Mack Follow

More information

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

To link to this article:   PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 06:24 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Crotophaga major (Greater Ani)

Crotophaga major (Greater Ani) Crotophaga major (Greater Ani) Family: Cuculidae (Cuckoos and Anis) Order: Cuculiformes (Cuckoos, Anis and Turacos) Class: Aves (Birds) Fig. 1. Greater ani, Crotophaga major. [http://www.birdforum.net/opus/greater_ani,

More information

Effects of interactive visitor encounters on the behaviour and welfare of animals commonly housed in Australian zoos

Effects of interactive visitor encounters on the behaviour and welfare of animals commonly housed in Australian zoos Effects of interactive visitor encounters on the behaviour and welfare of animals commonly housed in Australian zoos www.animalwelfare.net.au Lydia Acaralp-Rehnberg PhD candidate Supervisors: Dr Ian Bland,

More information

6 Month Progress Report. Cape vulture captive breeding and release programme Magaliesberg Mountains, South Africa. VulPro NPO

6 Month Progress Report. Cape vulture captive breeding and release programme Magaliesberg Mountains, South Africa. VulPro NPO 6 Month Progress Report Cape vulture captive breeding and release programme Magaliesberg Mountains, South Africa VulPro NPO Page Brooder and Incubator room construction 2 Cape Vulture captive bred chick

More information

Seven Nests of Rufescent Tiger-Heron (Tigrisoma lineatum)

Seven Nests of Rufescent Tiger-Heron (Tigrisoma lineatum) Seven Nests of Rufescent Tiger-Heron (Tigrisoma lineatum) Steven Furino and Mario Garcia Quesada Little is known about the nesting or breeding behaviour of Rufescent Tiger-Heron (Tigrisoma lineatum). Observations

More information

for Assistance Elise R. Shore a, Charles Burdsal a & Deanna K. Douglas b a Psychology Department, Wichita State University

for Assistance Elise R. Shore a, Charles Burdsal a & Deanna K. Douglas b a Psychology Department, Wichita State University This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 07:28 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

T HE behavior of a group of Common Rheas (Rhea americana) was

T HE behavior of a group of Common Rheas (Rhea americana) was SEXUAL AND AGONISTIC BEHAVIOR OF THE COMMON RHEA ROBERT J. RAIKOW T HE behavior of a group of Common Rheas (Rhea americana) was studied during 1962 and 1963 in large outdoor enclosures at the Detroit Zoological

More information

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

To link to this article:   PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 06:39 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Struthioniformes Family: Rheidae Scientific Name: Rhea pennata Common Name: Lesser Rhea

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Struthioniformes Family: Rheidae Scientific Name: Rhea pennata Common Name: Lesser Rhea Order: Struthioniformes Family: Rheidae Scientific Name: Rhea pennata Common Name: Lesser Rhea AZA Management: Green Yellow Red X None Photo (Male) with juvenile chicks: The female is generally duller

More information

110th CONGRESS 1st Session H. R. 1464

110th CONGRESS 1st Session H. R. 1464 HR 1464 IH 110th CONGRESS 1st Session H. R. 1464 To assist in the conservation of rare felids and rare canids by supporting and providing financial resources for the conservation programs of nations within

More information

February 1, 2018 Robert Gibbens, D.V.M. Director, Animal Welfare Operations USDA/APHIS/Animal Care

February 1, 2018 Robert Gibbens, D.V.M. Director, Animal Welfare Operations USDA/APHIS/Animal Care February 1, 2018 Robert Gibbens, D.V.M. Director, Animal Welfare Operations USDA/APHIS/Animal Care Via e-mail: ; acwest@aphis.usda.gov Dear Dr. Gibbens: I am writing on behalf of PETA to request that the

More information

Broom, D.M Causes of poor welfare in large animals during transport. Vet. Res, Commun., 27,

Broom, D.M Causes of poor welfare in large animals during transport. Vet. Res, Commun., 27, Broom, D.M. 2003. Causes of poor welfare in large animals during transport. Vet. Res, Commun., 27, 515-518. Pre-publication copy Causes of poor welfare in large animals during transport Donald M Broom

More information

International Journal of Scientific & Engineering Research, Volume 6, Issue 9, September ISSN

International Journal of Scientific & Engineering Research, Volume 6, Issue 9, September ISSN International Journal of Scientific & Engineering Research, Volume 6, Issue 9, September-2015 1799 NESTING ECOLOGY OF GORILLAS IN AFI MOUNTAIN WILDLIFE SANCTUARY, BOKI, CROSS RIVER STATE, NIGERIA 1 Ukpong,

More information

NESTING ECOLOGY OF GORILLAS IN AFI MOUNTAIN WILDLIFE SANCTUARY, BOKI, CROSS RIVER STATE, NIGERIA 1Ukpong, E. E.; 2Dike, M. C.; 3Roberts, U. U.

NESTING ECOLOGY OF GORILLAS IN AFI MOUNTAIN WILDLIFE SANCTUARY, BOKI, CROSS RIVER STATE, NIGERIA 1Ukpong, E. E.; 2Dike, M. C.; 3Roberts, U. U. ISSN 2320-9186 1 International Journal of Advance Research, IJOAR.org Volume 3, Issue 9, September 2015, Online: ISSN 2320-9186 NESTING ECOLOGY OF GORILLAS IN AFI MOUNTAIN WILDLIFE SANCTUARY, BOKI, CROSS

More information

R. Muñoz-Pulido a, L. M. Bautista b & J. C. Alonso b a Departamento de Biologí, a Animal, Facultad de Biologia,

R. Muñoz-Pulido a, L. M. Bautista b & J. C. Alonso b a Departamento de Biologí, a Animal, Facultad de Biologia, This article was downloaded by: [161.111.161.200] On: 26 July 2012, At: 07:16 Publisher: Taylor & Francis Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

ORIGINAL RESEARCH PAPER

ORIGINAL RESEARCH PAPER 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 ORIGINAL RESEARCH PAPER TIGERS STEREOTYPIC PACING AND ENRICHMENT Ivana

More information

ì<(sk$m)=bdddid< +^-Ä-U-Ä-U

ì<(sk$m)=bdddid< +^-Ä-U-Ä-U Suggested levels for Guided Reading, DRA, Lexile, and Reading Recovery are provided in the Pearson Scott Foresman Leveling Guide. Life Science Genre Expository nonfiction Comprehension Skills and Strategy

More information

Animal Study: Adelaide Zoo

Animal Study: Adelaide Zoo Animal Study: Adelaide Zoo Name: Animal: 16 1 Is this animal a social animal? Give reasons for your answer. Reflect on what you have learned about the animal you studied. If you were designing an enclosure

More information

Zurich Open Repository and Archive. Feeding live prey to zoo animals: response of zoo visitors in Switzerland

Zurich Open Repository and Archive. Feeding live prey to zoo animals: response of zoo visitors in Switzerland University of Zurich Zurich Open Repository and Archive Winterthurerstr. 190 CH-8057 Zurich http://www.zora.uzh.ch Year: 2010 Feeding live prey to zoo animals: response of zoo visitors in Switzerland Cottle,

More information

Abnormal Repetitive Behaviours

Abnormal Repetitive Behaviours Abnormal Repetitive Behaviours what do they tell us about zoo animal welfare? Ros Clubb 19th BIAZA Annual Research Conference July 2017 Collaborators Markus Gusset María Díez -León RVC CLICK Swiss Federal

More information

Anhinga anhinga (Anhinga or Snake-bird)

Anhinga anhinga (Anhinga or Snake-bird) Anhinga anhinga (Anhinga or Snake-bird) Family Anhingidae (Anhingas and Darters) Order: Pelecaniformes (Pelicans and Allied Waterbirds) Class: Aves (Birds) Fig. 1. Anhinga, Anhinga anhinga. [http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/accounts/anhinga_anhinga/,

More information

Module 2.4: Small Mammals Interpreting with Chinchillas

Module 2.4: Small Mammals Interpreting with Chinchillas Module 2.4: Small Mammals Interpreting with Chinchillas Interpreting with Chinchillas: The theme of your conversations may differ from group to group depending on the program, and the age of your audience.

More information

LANAnC22 - SQA Unit Code HA7G 04 Care for offspring and juvenile animals

LANAnC22 - SQA Unit Code HA7G 04 Care for offspring and juvenile animals LANAnC22 - SQA Unit Code HA7G 04 Overview This standard covers the care of offspring and juvenile animals. This may be as a result of breeding or through the purchase or introduction of juveniles. You

More information

Published online: 28 Apr 2015.

Published online: 28 Apr 2015. This article was downloaded by: [UQ Library] On: 11 July 2015, At: 03:36 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: 5 Howick Place, London,

More information

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc 1. The money in the kingdom of Florin consists of bills with the value written on the front, and pictures of members of the royal family on the back. To test the hypothesis that all of the Florinese $5

More information

CIWF Response to the Coalition for Sustainable Egg Supply Study April 2015

CIWF Response to the Coalition for Sustainable Egg Supply Study April 2015 CIWF Response to the Coalition for Sustainable Egg Supply Study April 2015 The Coalition for Sustainable Egg Supply study seeks to understand the sustainability impacts of three laying hen housing systems

More information

The Road Lesser Traveled: A Map to Rheabilitation and Conservation By Kenna Mokobi

The Road Lesser Traveled: A Map to Rheabilitation and Conservation By Kenna Mokobi The Road Lesser Traveled: A Map to Rheabilitation and Conservation By Kenna Mokobi Sophomore College: Parks and Peoples with Professor Bill Durham 16th October 2016 Stanford University Picture 1: A lesser

More information

Cristiano S. de Azevedo & Robert J. Young

Cristiano S. de Azevedo & Robert J. Young Shyness and boldness in greater rheas Rhea americana Linnaeus (Rheifor ormes, Rheidae): the effects ects of antipredator training on the personality of the birds Cristiano S. de Azevedo & Robert J. Young

More information

Internship Report: Raptor Conservation in Bulgaria

Internship Report: Raptor Conservation in Bulgaria Internship Report: Raptor Conservation in Bulgaria All photos credited Natasha Peters, David Izquierdo, or Vladimir Dobrev reintroduction programme in Bulgaria Life History Size: 47-55 cm / 105-129 cm

More information

Behaviour of Hens in Cages

Behaviour of Hens in Cages Behaviour of Hens in Cages - a pilot study using video tapes A report for the Rural Industries Research and Development Corporation by Clare Rudkin and Geoff D. Stewart August RIRDC Publication No / RIRDC

More information

Do Visitors Affect Zebra Behavior in Zoos?

Do Visitors Affect Zebra Behavior in Zoos? University of Connecticut DigitalCommons@UConn Honors Scholar Theses Honors Scholar Program Spring 5-2-2014 Do Visitors Affect Zebra Behavior in Zoos? Christine Conte christineconte23@gmail.com Follow

More information

Dangerous Wild Animals (Northern Ireland) Order Guidance on the keeping of Ostrich and Emus

Dangerous Wild Animals (Northern Ireland) Order Guidance on the keeping of Ostrich and Emus Dangerous Wild Animals (Northern Ireland) Order 2004 Guidance on the keeping of Ostrich and Emus www.ehsni.gov.uk Guidance on the keeping of Ostrich and Emus 1. Species Names 2. Additional information

More information

Also known as the little corella, short-billed corella.

Also known as the little corella, short-billed corella. Bare-eyed cockatoo Cacatua sanguinea Indonesia Also known as the little corella, short-billed corella. Bare-eyed cockatoos are medium sized white cockatoos with short white recumbent crests. The eye rings

More information

The 1999 EU Hens Directive bans the conventional battery cage from 2012.

The 1999 EU Hens Directive bans the conventional battery cage from 2012. PS/MJ/BR9718 April 2002 ENRICHED CAGES FOR EGG-LAYING HENS B R I E F I N G EU ban on the conventional battery cage The 1999 EU Hens Directive bans the conventional battery cage from 2012. The ban is well

More information

Silverback Male Presence and Group Stability in Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla)

Silverback Male Presence and Group Stability in Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) Brief Report Folia Primatol 753 Received: August 16, 2002 DOI: 10.1159/0000XXXXX Accepted after revision: October 30, 2002 Silverback Male Presence and Group Stability in Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla)

More information

STANDING COMMITTEE OF THE EUROPEAN CONVENTION FOR THE PROTECTION OF ANIMALS KEPT FOR FARMING PURPOSES

STANDING COMMITTEE OF THE EUROPEAN CONVENTION FOR THE PROTECTION OF ANIMALS KEPT FOR FARMING PURPOSES STANDING COMMITTEE OF THE EUROPEAN CONVENTION FOR THE PROTECTION OF ANIMALS KEPT FOR FARMING PURPOSES WELFARE RECOMMENDATION CONCERNING RATITES (Ostriches, Emus and Rheas) adopted by the Standing Committee

More information

Geoffroy s Cat: Biodiversity Research Project

Geoffroy s Cat: Biodiversity Research Project Geoffroy s Cat: Biodiversity Research Project Viet Nguyen Conservation Biology BES 485 Geoffroy s Cat Geoffroy s Cat (Leopardus geoffroyi) are small, little known spotted wild cat found native to the central

More information

Husbandry Guidelines Name Species Prepared by

Husbandry Guidelines Name Species Prepared by Husbandry Guidelines Name Species Prepared by 1. ACQUISITION AND ACCLIMATIZATION Status of wild population Status current captive population Sources of birds Acclimatization procedures Weighing Feeding

More information

Effects of a Pre-Molt Calcium and Low-Energy Molt Program on Laying Hen Behavior During and Post-Molt

Effects of a Pre-Molt Calcium and Low-Energy Molt Program on Laying Hen Behavior During and Post-Molt Animal Industry Report AS 655 ASL R2446 2009 Effects of a Pre-Molt Calcium and Low-Energy Molt Program on Laying Hen Behavior During and Post-Molt Emily R. Dickey Anna K. Johnson George Brant Rob Fitzgerald

More information

INDEX ACTH, 27, 41 adoption of cats, 76, 135, 137, 150 adrenocorticotropic hormone. See ACTH affiliative behaviours, 2, 5, 7, 18, 66 African wild cat,

INDEX ACTH, 27, 41 adoption of cats, 76, 135, 137, 150 adrenocorticotropic hormone. See ACTH affiliative behaviours, 2, 5, 7, 18, 66 African wild cat, INDEX ACTH, 27, 41 adoption of cats, 76, 135, 137, 150 adrenocorticotropic hormone. See ACTH affiliative s, 2, 5, 7, 18, 66 African wild cat, 1, 27, 47, 181 aggression, 2, 4, 12, 16, 18, 29, 30, 66, 76,

More information

Housing for Health, Wellness and Success: Standards for Facility Design and Environment. What is a healthy environment made of?

Housing for Health, Wellness and Success: Standards for Facility Design and Environment. What is a healthy environment made of? Housing for Health, Wellness and Success: Standards for Facility Design and Environment Kate Hurley UC Davis Koret Shelter Medicine Program www.sheltermedicine.com www.facebook.com/sheltermedicine What

More information

Proponent: Switzerland, as Depositary Government, at the request of the Animals Committee (prepared by New Zealand)

Proponent: Switzerland, as Depositary Government, at the request of the Animals Committee (prepared by New Zealand) Transfer of Caspian Snowcock Tetraogallus caspius from Appendix I to Appendix II Ref. CoP16 Prop. 18 Proponent: Switzerland, as Depositary Government, at the request of the Animals Committee (prepared

More information

Modification of Laying Hen Cages to Improve Behavior

Modification of Laying Hen Cages to Improve Behavior Modification of Laying Hen Cages to Improve Behavior MICHAEL C. APPLEBY1 Institute of Ecology and Resource Management, University of Edinburgh, West Mains Road, Edinburgh EH9 3JG, United Kingdom ABSTRACT

More information

Dimethyl anthranilate based repellents affect cage pecking and feather condition of laying hens

Dimethyl anthranilate based repellents affect cage pecking and feather condition of laying hens Dimethyl anthranilate based repellents affect cage pecking and feather condition of laying hens Animal Science Days September 21 st 2017 Uroš Šraj 2, Dušan Terčič 1, Dušanka Jordan 1, Mojca Pestotnik 1,

More information

Measuring zoo animal welfare: Theory and practice

Measuring zoo animal welfare: Theory and practice 1 Hill 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Hill, S.P. and Broom, D.M. 2009. Measuring zoo animal welfare: theory and practice. Zoo Biol., 28, 531-544. DOI: 10.1002/zoo.20276 Pre-publication copy Measuring zoo animal welfare:

More information

Northern Copperhead Updated: April 8, 2018

Northern Copperhead Updated: April 8, 2018 Interpretation Guide Northern Copperhead Updated: April 8, 2018 Status Danger Threats Population Distribution Habitat Diet Size Longevity Social Family Units Reproduction Our Animals Scientific Name Least

More information

Overview LANCTB1. Observe, assess and respond to the behaviour of dogs. Observe, assess and respond to the behaviour of dogs

Overview LANCTB1. Observe, assess and respond to the behaviour of dogs. Observe, assess and respond to the behaviour of dogs Overview This standard covers observing, assessing and responding to the behaviour of dogs. The standard involves understanding the normal behaviour of the type of dog that you are working with and observing

More information

Proposed Draft Australian Animal Welfare Standards And Guidelines For Poultry. Submission from the Australian Veterinary Association Ltd

Proposed Draft Australian Animal Welfare Standards And Guidelines For Poultry. Submission from the Australian Veterinary Association Ltd Proposed Draft Australian Animal Welfare Standards And Guidelines For Poultry Submission from the Australian Veterinary Association Ltd 1 24 February 2018 Introduction The Australian Veterinary Association

More information

Coalition for a Sustainable Egg Supply Richard Blatchford University of California, Davis

Coalition for a Sustainable Egg Supply Richard Blatchford University of California, Davis Coalition for a Sustainable Egg Supply Richard Blatchford University of California, Davis Growing public interest in food production Concern about hen welfare, focusing on conventional cages Overview Egg

More information

Tigers Stereotypic Pacing and Enrichment

Tigers Stereotypic Pacing and Enrichment Annual Research & Review in Biology 4(10): 1544-1550, 2014 SCIENCEDOMAIN international www.sciencedomain.org Tigers Stereotypic Pacing and Enrichment Ivana Gardiánová 1* and Petra Bolechová 2,3 1 Demonstrational

More information

Procnias averano (Bearded Bellbird)

Procnias averano (Bearded Bellbird) Procnias averano (Bearded Bellbird) Family: Cotingidae (Bellbirds and Cotingas) Order: Passeriformes (Perching Birds) Class: Aves (Birds) Fig. 1. Bearded bellbird, Procnias averano. [http://www.oiseaux.net/photos/steve.garvie/bearded.bellbird.5.html

More information

Animal Welfare Assessment Transfers Checklist

Animal Welfare Assessment Transfers Checklist Animal Welfare Assessment Transfers Checklist Our Animal Welfare Commitment The believes that consideration of an animal s welfare must include its physical, physiological and mental state and that good

More information

REARING LAYING HENS IN A BARN SYSTEM WITHOUT BEAK TRIMMING: THE RONDEEL EXAMPLE

REARING LAYING HENS IN A BARN SYSTEM WITHOUT BEAK TRIMMING: THE RONDEEL EXAMPLE REARING LAYING HENS IN A BARN SYSTEM WITHOUT BEAK TRIMMING: THE RONDEEL EXAMPLE BACKGROUND: BEAK TRIMMING AND FEATHER PECKING IN LAYING HENS Injurious feather pecking is a major welfare problem in laying

More information

Multiple broods from a hole in the wall: breeding Red-and-yellow Barbets Trachyphonus erythrocephalus in southeast Sudan

Multiple broods from a hole in the wall: breeding Red-and-yellow Barbets Trachyphonus erythrocephalus in southeast Sudan Scopus 29: 11 15, December 2009 Multiple broods from a hole in the wall: breeding Red-and-yellow Barbets Trachyphonus erythrocephalus in southeast Sudan Marc de Bont Summary Nesting and breeding behaviour

More information

Nova Scotia Service Dog Assessment

Nova Scotia Service Dog Assessment Nova Scotia Service Dog Assessment The purpose of the service dog assessment is to ensure the service dog demonstrates appropriate public behavior, proper disposition, and is unobtrusive in public places.

More information

NOTES ON BREEDING SATYR TRAGOPAN TRAGOPAN SATYRA AT SRI CHAMARAJENDRA ZOOLOGICAL GARDENS, MYSORE

NOTES ON BREEDING SATYR TRAGOPAN TRAGOPAN SATYRA AT SRI CHAMARAJENDRA ZOOLOGICAL GARDENS, MYSORE NOTES ON BREEDING SATYR TRAGOPAN TRAGOPAN SATYRA AT SRI CHAMARAJENDRA ZOOLOGICAL GARDENS, MYSORE ABSTRACT Satyr tragopan Tragopan satyra has been classified as Near Threatened in the IUCN Red List of Threatened

More information

Big Cat Rescue Presents. Tigrina or Oncilla

Big Cat Rescue Presents. Tigrina or Oncilla Big Cat Rescue Presents Tigrina or Oncilla 1 Tigrina or Oncilla Big Cat Rescue 12802 Easy Street Tampa, Florida 33625 www.bigcatrescue.org Common Name: Oncilla Kingdom: Animalia Phylum: Chordata (Vertebrata)

More information