Health-dependent vulnerability to predation affects escape responses of unguarded chinstrap penguin chicks

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Health-dependent vulnerability to predation affects escape responses of unguarded chinstrap penguin chicks"

Transcription

1 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (6) 6: DOI.7/s ORIGINAL ARTICLE Health-dependent vulnerability to predation affects escape responses of unguarded chinstrap penguin chicks J. Martín & L. de Neve & V. Polo & J. A. Fargallo & M. Soler Received: 9 December 5 /Revised: 8 March 6 / Accepted: May 6 / Published online: June 6 # Springer-Verlag 6 Abstract Predators may select more often to attack the more vulnerable prey or those with an inferior health status. Thus, prey should be able to assess their own vulnerability to predation and modify their antipredatory behavior accordingly. When approached by predator skuas, unguarded penguin chicks flee short distances, and usually aggregate in dense packs, but there is a clear interindividual variability in their responses under similar conditions. We hypothesized that this variability in escape responses might be related to the perceived vulnerability to predation of each individual chick. We simulated predator attacks to chinstrap penguin chicks and analyzed the sources of variation in their escape response, such as the presence of adults or the density of other chicks, and the sex, age, body condition, and health status of responding chicks. Chicks allowed shorter approach distances when they had a better health condition (i.e., a greater T-cell-mediated immunity, CMI), when they were younger, and when the density of adults around was higher. Sex and density of other chicks were not important. Similarly, chicks fled from the experimenter to longer distances when they had a lower CMI and when the density of adults was lower. Therefore, Communicated by C. Brown J. Martín (*) : L. de Neve : V. Polo : J. A. Fargallo Departamento de Ecología Evolutiva, Museo Nacional de Ciencias Naturales, C.S.I.C., José Gutiérrez Abascal, 86 Madrid, Spain Jose.Martin@mncn.csic.es M. Soler Departamento de Biología Animal y Ecología, Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad de Granada, 87 Granada, Spain escape characteristics of chicks depended on the presence of adults that can deter predators and on the healthdependent vulnerability of chicks. Keywords Prey vulnerability. Penguins. Health condition. Immunocompetence. Fear. Antipredatory behavior Introduction When predation risk changes, prey animals should be able to modify the magnitude of their antipredatory behavior to the actual level of threat because antipredatory responses can be costly in terms of loss of time and energy (Lima and Dill 99; Ydenberg and Dill 986). Moreover, recent studies have shown that predators may respond to changing vulnerability of their prey on quite a subtle level, selecting more often to attack the more vulnerable individuals (Cresswell and Quinn ; Quinn and Cresswell ). Thus, prey should be able to assess their own vulnerability to predation and modify their antipredatory behavior accordingly. A prey can theoretically assess vulnerability on the basis of many measures such as microhabitat characteristics, predator behavior, group size, or activity (Smith and Belk ; Stankowich and Blumstein 5). Prey vulnerability should be highest when animals are under energetic stress because they are forced to allocate fewer resources to antipredatory behavior (Lima 998).For example,by foraging in more risky habitats or reducing vigilance rate, prey may increase intake rate, but may also increase their vulnerability (Caraco et al. 98; Quinn and Cresswell ). Vulnerability of individuals within groups may decrease when group size increases because of the dilution effect, where the risk of any one individual being selected is reduced simply by being in a

2 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (6) 6: larger group (Roberts 996), or because animals may rely on collective detection of a threat via other wary animals in the group (e.g., Creswell et al. ). In addition, predators are usually considered to prey upon individuals of inferior phenotypic quality because these would be easier to subdue and capture, although this hypothesis has only rather rarely been tested (Temple 986; Møller and Erritzøe ; Pole et al. ). These studies found that some raptors differentially preyed upon mammals that were heavily parasitized (Temple 986), and that passerine birds falling prey to feral cats have small spleens, which implies that avian prey often have a poor health status (Møller and Erritzøe ). These data provided indirect evidence that predators preferentially prey on individuals in poor condition that suffer from parasitism and disease and with an inferior health status. This selection might be explained as simply a result of a differential hunting success (Fitzgibbon 99; Funston et al. ), but predators could also be able to identify and choose to attack these most vulnerable prey (Cresswell and Quinn ; Quinn and Cresswell ). In any case, health-dependent vulnerability of prey should also affect their antipredator responses. In the penguin breeding colonies, predation by skuas (Catharacta spp.) is the major cause of chick mortality (Hunter 99; Young 99; Emslie et al. 995). One of the parents uses to provide continuous protection to young chicks (Viñuela et al. 995), but relatively older chicks are often left unattended by both parents while they forage in the sea, and chicks often form more or less dense aggregations until emancipation (Davis 98; Williams 995; Penteriani et al. ). Chicks are still vulnerable to predation by skuas during this chick aggregation stage (Davis 98),and grouping may function to provide protection from predators and/or from inclement weather (Davis 98; Lawless et al. ; Besnard et al. ; Le Bohec et al. 5). However, older chicks are very difficult to subdue by skuas because these older chicks are much heavier than skuas and can defend themselves very aggressively (Young 99). Skuas, then, behave more like scavengers and tend to feed on carcasses of penguins (Norman et al. 99). Nevertheless, skuas are often observed harassing chicks within the aggregations and can still kill some chicks (Davis 98; Young 99). When approached by skuas, unguarded chicks of the chinstrap penguin, Pygoscelis antarctica, use to flee short distances and usually aggregate in dense packs of chicks, which are supposed to decrease predation risk (Cresswell 99a; Young 99). However, there is an apparently clear variability in the escape responses of different individual chicks under similar conditions. Some individuals flee sooner than others, and even some individuals do not flee at all but standstill and face the approaching skua, threatening it with open bill and loud shouts (personal observation). We hypothesized that this variability might be related to the perceived vulnerability to predation of each individual chick, which might depend not only on the probability of being defended by adults from an attack, and on the dilution effect of being in a group with other chicks, but also on the statedependent ability of each individual chick to defend itself. In this paper, we simulated predatory attacks and examined the escape response of chinstrap penguin chicks during the chick aggregation phase in an Antarctic breeding colony. We analyzed the sources of variation in the escape response of chicks, such as the presence of some adult penguins around, or the density of other chicks, and examined whether the characteristics of these responses could be state-dependent, i. e., related to the chicks health status and body condition, independently of age, sex, and the presence of conspecifics. Materials and methods Study area and general methods We performed the study at the Vapour Col chinstrap penguin colony (, breeding pairs) on Deception Island, South Shetlands (6 S, 6 W) during the austral summer. In this colony, penguin eggs and chicks are commonly predated by Subantarctic skua (C. antarctica). During the breeding season, we followed up a total of penguin nests ( nests in each of subcolonies) from incubation until the chick aggregation phase. The chicks of these nests were individually marked with insulation tape and later with numbered metallic rings on one flipper. Flipper-bands were removed at the end of the study to avoid their potential detrimental costs to chicks (Jackson and Wilson ). Because we had visited daily the nest to control whether the eggs had hatched, we knew the exact age of each of these chicks at the moment of carrying out this study. Sex was determined by molecular procedures using DNA extracted from blood taken from the external foot web from young chicks (Ellegren 996). Escape behavior The experiment was done shortly after the guard phase, when adults left chicks unguarded and these started to form aggregations (Viñuela et al. 996; Penteriani et al. ), and before independence. Unguarded penguin chicks found in aggregations were between 8 and 8 days of age (mean± SE=.±. days, n=68). We searched the area between 9 and 6 h (GMT) by walking slowly between the groups of chicks until a marked penguin chick was sighted using binoculars. We noted without disturbing it the number of other chicks and adult penguins in a -m circle around the focal chick. This ratio was chosen after some trials because it allowed us to easily count

3 78 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (6) 6: individuals just before the attack and because it was considered to include individuals actually affected by the attack (i.e., a larger circle would include individuals too far from the focal chick that very often did not respond to the approach). Then, we approached chicks by simulating an approaching skua by walking directly and fast (ca. m/ min) toward the focal chick. Previous studies have suggested that penguins and other animals do not actually respond to the presence of a human but to the speed of approach of a large object (Burger and Gochfeld 98; Van Heezik and Seddon 99; Yorio and Boersma 99), which would be similar to the approach of a predator. Thus, presumably, a rapid direct approach should be considered by penguins as a higher risk of predation because it should indicate a high probability of a serious attack (Martín et al. ). We are confident that with this procedure, we simulated a predator attack because observations of skua approaches to groups of chicks indicated that these reacted with a similar antipredatory behavior to natural skua predators than to the human experimenter (Martín et al. ; unpublished data). Similarly, many other animals react to humans as if they were potential predators (Frid and Dill ). To avoid confounding effects that may affect risk perception (e.g., Burger and Gochfeld 98), the same person wearing the same clothing performed all approaches in a similar way, walking at the same medium speed, while another person recorded the chicks behavior. Also, because approach distance in birds may be dependent on intruder starting distance (Blumstein ), we started approaches at the same distance in all cases (approximately 5 m). This starting distance was established after some preliminary trials. Penguins typically made a short flight at relatively fast speed, and then stopped, or later walked slowly. We noted the approach distance as the distance between the penguin and the observer when the penguin first moved (straight-line measured to the nearest.5 m), and the distance fled as the distance across which a penguin fled away from the observer until first stopping (Ydenberg and Dill 986; Martín and López ; Martín et al. ). Body condition and cell-mediated immunity Immediately after the simulated attack, we captured the focal chick with a net and measured the flipper length (mean±se=9.±. cm; range=7.6.5 cm) with a rule to the nearest mm, bill length (9.±. mm; range=..8 mm) with a caliper to the nearest. mm, and body mass (.±. kg; range=.. kg) with a Pesola spring balance to the nearest 5 g. After removing variation with age, male chicks were significantly heavier than females (age: F,65 =., P=.7; sex: F,65 =.69, P<.), had significantly longer flippers (age: F,65 =.85, P=.5; sex: F,65 =., P=.5), and significantly longer bills than females (age: F,65 =9.7, P<.; sex: F,65 =5.6, P=.). Body condition was estimated as the residuals from an ANCOVA with log-body mass as the dependent variable, sex as a factor, and log-flipper length and log-bill length as covariates (R =.7, F,6 =.9, P<.) (Tella et al. ). To assess the health condition of chicks, we used a delayed-type hypersensitivity test; we measured the cellmediated immune (CMI) response by challenging the immune system through the subcutaneous injection of a novel mitogen (phytohemagglutinin, PHA-P, Sigma) (Smits et al. 999). PHA is a mitogen that produces a local swelling due to a prominent perivascular accumulation of T-lymphocytes followed by macrophage infiltration (Smits et al. 999). This method is considered to be a reliable method of measuring CMI (reviewed by Norris and Evans ). We injected.5 mg PHA dissolved in. ml of physiological saline solution (Bausch and Lomb) in the right external foot web (Moreno et al. 998; Tella et al. ). The thickness of the foot web was measured three times at the injection site with a digital caliper (. mm) just before injection. The repeatability of the three measurements calculated as the intraclass correlation coefficient (Lessells and Boag 987) was high (r=.97, F 6,9 =86.6, P<.). The chicks were liberated at the exact capture point and recaptured after h (± h) after injection to measure again three times the thickness of the foot web with the same procedure (repeatability: r=.99, F 6,9 =57., P<.). We used the mean difference in thickness between the day of injection and the following day as an estimate of CMI (Moreno et al. 998; Smits et al. 999). Data analysis To investigate the relationships between escape characteristics (approach distance and distance fled) and chick characteristics (sex, age, body condition, and CMI) and the initial density of other chicks and adults around, we used general linear modeling (GLM) (Crawley 99) with normal error distribution. Because some of the explanatory variables may covary, we used a forward stepwise procedure. We tested the variation explained by each variable in separate models, the variable explaining more variance was added to the model, and the significance of the remaining variables was tested again until no additional variable reached significance. The dependent variables were log-transformed to ensure normality. Residuals from the final model were normally distributed. Results Measures of CMI ranged between. and. mm (mean± SE=.±.6 mm). A GLM model (R =.6, F,57 =.7,

4 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (6) 6: P=.6) showed that the CMI of chicks was significantly related to age (F,57 =7., P<.) and body condition (F,57 =., P=.) but was independent on sex (F,57 =., P=.7). Thus, both older male and female chicks and with a higher body condition had a greater CMI. Approach distances of penguin chicks varied between and m (mean±se=.9±. m). Variability in approach distances was explained by the chick CMI, age, and initial density of adults m around (GLM model, R =., F,6 =6., P<.) (Table ). Chicks had shorter approach distances when they had a greater CMI (Fig. a), when they were younger (Fig. b), and when the density of adults around was higher (Fig. c). However, neither sex, body condition, nor initial density of other chicks m around was retained in the final model. Distances fled by penguin chicks varied between and 6 m (mean±se=.5±. m). Variability in distances fled was related to the chick CMI and the initial density of adults m around (GLM model, R =., F,6 =., P=.9) (Table ). Chicks fled to longer distances when they had a lower CMI (Fig. a) and when the density of adults was lower (Fig. b). Neither age, sex, body condition, nor initial density of other chicks was retained in the final model. Discussions Our results indicated that penguin chicks differed in the characteristics of their escape response when approached by a simulated predator. Variability in escape behavior was partly explained by the social environment (i.e., the presence of some adults around). Adult penguins are effective in deterring skua attacks, as they normally respond to the skua presence with aggressive defensive behavior until the skua is expelled (Davis 98; Young 99; personal observation). This does not imply that adults close to chicks aggregations are defending all chicks, but chicks might assess that the adult presence lowers their vulnerability to predation, and, then, chicks may decrease the intensity of their escape behavior accordingly. This would reduce the costs of an unnecessarily high antipredatory response. In contrast, the density of other chicks around did not modify the escape response of chicks. This suggests that the possible dilution effect (Cresswell 99a; Roberts 996) of a larger number of chicks per se in aggregations might not be considered by penguin chicks to increase safety. Otherwise, chicks within a larger or denser group should have tolerated closer predator approaches, as individual risk would be reduced (Fernández-Juricic et al. ). It remains possible that the dilution effect of a large number of available prey would be important to prey species where predators attack by surprise. If a predator simply attacks any individual prey, or those prey that exceed a minimum vulnerability level, a prey animal s predation risk will be determined by its absolute vulnerability, which might depend on group size. However, in prey species where predators can examine available prey before choosing to attack the most vulnerable individual, dilution of risk for an individual prey might depend on the relative availability of other more vulnerable prey in the group (Cresswell and Quinn ) and not just on the total number of individuals in the group. This might be the case for penguin chicks and skuas, where skuas often approach to the groups of chicks by walking and examine chicks before launching an actual attack (personal observation). Chick age was another determinant of their approach distance. If the ability to defend from an attack would depend on body size (Young 99), older (=larger) chicks would be less vulnerable to predation and might delay their escape response. However, in contrast to what is expected from age-related differences in body size, younger chicks reacted significantly later to the simulated predator. This Table Results of GLM analyses on how a penguin chick escape behavior (approach and flight distances) was affected by the chick s CMI response and age, and the density of adults around Variable source Adj. SS df F P Coeff. SE Coeff. Approach distance Intercept CMI response <...7 Age Adult density Error.66 6 Flight distance Intercept <..5. CMI response Adult density Error

5 78 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (6) 6: Approach distance (m),5,5 Immune response (CMI) a b c Age (days) Adults m around Fig. Relationships between the approach distance of penguin chicks in response to a simulated predator attack and a the T-cell-mediated immune response (CMI), b the age of chicks, and c the initial density of adults m around the focal chick might suggest that the ability, or the need, to recognize and react to predator approaches develops with age. When chicks are young, at least one of the parents is always in the nest guarding the chicks from approaching predators (Viñuela et al. 995); thus, young chicks might not need to show escape responses, whereas when older chicks are left unguarded, they need to defend by themselves and should escape when a predator approaches. Our results further suggested that penguin chicks with a worse health state, as indicated by their T-cell immune response, were more frightened by our approach, which affects their escape response characteristics (i.e., they fled sooner and ran farther). Some studies have shown a link Distance fled (m) Distance fled (m) 6 5,5,5 Immune response (CMI) b 6 5 a Adults m around Fig. Relationships between the distance fled by penguin chicks in response to a simulated predator attack and a the T-cell-mediated immune response (CMI), and b the initial density of adults m around the focal chick between risk-taking behavior under the threat of predation and personality traits like fear (e.g., Quinn and Cresswell 5), but the interaction between these behaviors and health was almost unknown. Our results may be explained if, as occurs in other bird species, antipredator behavior performance was affected by their nutritional condition and presence of disease (Lindström et al. ). Similarly, lizards with a poor health state (i.e., lower CMI response) were shier when confronting with a simulated predator (López et al. 5). Other indirect evidences of this relationship between health state and shyness have been found in experiments on fear responses under chronic stress. Stress may increase the levels of corticosteroids, with the concomitant immunosuppressive effects of these hormones (Cohn 997). Thus, an index of fear in rats was positively correlated with plasma levels of corticosterone (Gamallo et al. 986). In hens, tonic immobility, a specific fear-related response, was prolonged after the basal plasma levels of corticosterone were artificially increased (Jones et al. 988). Penguin chicks with a worse health state might have suffered some previous stress situations such as a high exposure to predation risk or social harassment by adults, or sibling competition for food. For example, experimental

6 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (6) 6: feeding experiments have shown that northern bobwhite chicks in poor nutritional condition show a weaker CMI (Lochmiller et al. 99). Also, our results indicated a positive relationship between CMI and body condition in chinstrap penguin chicks, as it also occurs in other penguin species (Tella et al. ). Hatching asynchrony is common in penguins (Moreno et al. 99), larger penguin chicks of double broods get most of the food provided by the parents (Blanco et al. 996), and CMI differs between larger and smaller fledglings, with smaller siblings tending to have a lower CMI (Tella et al. ). Also, it remains possible that, if the immune response were heritable (e.g., Brinkhof et al. 999), chicks with a lower CMI might be the sons of late breeders, which had poorer health and a lower CMI than early breeders (Moreno et al. 998). Predators should prey upon individuals of inferior phenotypic quality (Temple 986; Møller and Erritzøe ). Thus, several species of passerine birds falling prey to cats had smaller spleens, and, thus, a lower immunocompetence as well than other conspecifics (Møller and Erritzøe, but see Smith and Hunt for a review). Recent studies have suggested that, when predators are able to monitor a large number of potential prey, they might use subtle variations in behavior of different prey to select them according to their vulnerability (Cresswell and Quinn ; Quinn and Cresswell ). Thus, predators would avoid predictably unsuccessful chases. It is likely that if health state affects general antipredatory behavior performance (Lindström et al. ), the ability of poor condition penguin chicks to defend themselves may be lower, forcing them to flee sooner and for longer. Only penguin chicks in good condition may delay fleeing. Therefore, the characteristics of escape behavior of chicks might be considered as an honest signal of condition (Grafen 99; Vega-Redondo and Hasson 99), Skuas and other avian predators might assess that individual penguin chicks that escape relatively later or face the predator will be more difficult to capture. Similarly, distress calls of some larks, which are used to discourage predators (Cresswell 99b), reflect their health condition (CMI response and body condition) and, thus, may signal their ability to escape (Laiolo et al. ). Further experiments are clearly needed to analyze whether skuas are able to assess prey vulnerability when attacking penguin chicks. Acknowledgements We thank JL Tella and JL Quinn for the helpful comments on the manuscript. This study was based on the Spanish Army Base Gabriel de Castilla ; transport to and from Deception Island was provided by the Spanish Navy ship Las Palmas. We thank the cooperation offered by all participants in the Spanish Antarctic campaign / and especially to JC, the man with long sideburns, for his persistent companionship. Financial support was provided by the Spanish Antarctic Program, MCYT project BOS -7. The manipulations and experimental procedures were approved by the Scientific Committee for Antarctic Research (SCAR). References Besnard A, Gimenez O, Lebreton JD () A model for the evolution of crèching behaviour in gulls. Evol Ecol 6: 89 5 Blanco DE, Yorio P, Boersma PD (996) Feeding behaviour, size asymmetry, and food distribution in Magellanic penguin (Spheniscus magellanicus) chicks. Auk :96 98 Blumstein DT () Flight initiation distance in birds is dependent on intruder starting distance. J Wildl Manage 67: Brinkhof MWG, Heeb P, Kolliker M, Richner H (999) Immunocompetence of nestling great tits in relation to rearing environment and parentage. Proc R Soc Lond B 66:5 Burger J, Gochfeld M (98) Discrimination of the threat of direct versus tangential approach to the nest by incubating herring and great black-backed gulls. J Comp Physiol Psychol 95: Caraco T, Martindale S, Pulliam HR (98) Avian time budgets and distance to cover. Auk 97: Cohn LA (997) Glucocorticoids as immunosuppressive agents. Sem Vet Med Surg Small Anim :5 56 Crawley MJ (99) GLIM for ecologists. Blackwell, Oxford, UK Cresswell W (99a) Flocking is an effective anti-predation strategy in redshanks, Tringa totanus. Anim Behav 7: Cresswell W (99b) Song as a pursuit-deterrent signal, and its occurrence relative to other anti-predation behaviours of skylark (Alauda arvensis) on attack by merlins (Falco columbarius). Behav Ecol Sociobiol :7 Cresswell W, Quinn JL () Faced with a choice, sparrowhawks more often attack the more vulnerable prey group. Oikos :7 76 Creswell W, Hilton GM, Ruxton GD () Evidence for a rule governing the avoidance of superfluous escape flights. Proc R Soc Lond B 67:7 77 Davis LS (98) Crèching behaviour of Adèlie penguin chicks (Pygoscelis adeliae). NZ J Zool 9:79 86 Ellegren H (996) First gene on the avian W chromosome (CHD) provides a tag for universal sexing of non-ratite birds. Proc R Soc Lond B 6:65 6 Emslie SD, Karnovsky N, Trivelpiece W (995) Avian predation at penguin colonies on King George Island, Antarctica. Wilson Bull 7:7 7 Fernández-Juricic E, Jiménez MD, Lucas E () Factors affecting intra- and inter-specific variations in the difference between alert distances and flight distances for birds in forested habitats. Can J Zool 8: Fitzgibbon CD (99) Why do hunting cheetahs prefer male gazelles? Anim Behav :87 85 Frid A, Dill LM () Human-caused disturbance stimuli as a form of predation risk. Conserv Ecol 6: Funston PJ, Mills MGL, Biggs HC () Factors affecting the hunting success of male and female lions in the Kruger National Park. J Zool 5:9 Gamallo A, Villanua A, Trancho G, Fraile A (986) Stress adaptation and adrenal activity in isolated and crowded rats. Physiol Behav 6:7 Grafen A (99) Biological signals as handicaps. J Theor Biol :57 56 Hunter S (99) The impact of avian predator scavengers on king penguins Aptenodytes patagonicus chicks at Marion Island. Ibis : 5 Jackson S, Wilson RP () The potential costs of flipper-bands to penguins. Funct Ecol 6: 8 Jones RB, Beuving G, Blokhuis HJ (988) Tonic immobility and heterophil/lymphocyte responses of the domestic fowl to corticosterone infusion. Physiol Behav :9 5

7 78 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (6) 6: Laiolo P, Tella JL, Carrete M, Serrano D, López G () Distress calls may honestly signal bird quality to predators. Proc R Soc Lond B 7:S5 S55 (Suppl) Lawless RM, Buttemer WA, Astheimer LB, Kerry KR () The influence of thermoregulatory demand on contact crèching behaviour in Adèlie Penguin chicks. J Therm Biol 6: Le Bohec C, Gauthier-Clerc M, Le Maho Y (5) The adaptive significance of crèches in the king penguin. Anim Behav 7: Lessells CM, Boag PT (987) Unrepeteable repeatabilities: a common mistake. Auk :6 Lima SL (998) Stress and decision making under the risk of predation: recent developments from behavioral, reproductive and ecological perspectives. Adv Stud Behav 7: 5 9 Lima SL, Dill LM (99) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68:69 6 Lindström KM, Van de Veen IT, Lagault BA, Lundström JO () Activity and predator escape performance of common greenfinches Carduelis chloris infected with Sindbis virus. Ardea 9: Lochmiller RL, Vestey MR, Boren JC (99) Relationship between protein nutritional status and immunocompetence in northern bobwhite chicks. Auk :5 5 López P, Hawlena D, Polo V, Amo L, Martín J (5) Sources of individual shy-bold variations in antipredatory behaviour of male Iberian rock-lizards. Anim Behav 69: 9 Martín J, López P () Fleeing to unsafe refuges: effects of conspicuousness and refuge safety on the escape decisions of the lizard Psammodromus algirus. Can J Zool 78:65 7 Martín J, de Neve L, Fargallo JA, Polo V, Soler M () Factors affecting the escape behavior of juvenile chinstrap penguins, Pygoscelis antarctica, in response to human disturbance. Polar Biol 7: Møller AP, Erritzøe J () Predation against birds with low immunocompetence Oecologia :5 5 Moreno J, Carrascal LM, Sanz JJ, Amat JA, Cuervo JJ (99) Hatching asynchrony, sibling hierarchies and brood reduction in the chinstrap penguin Pygoscelis antarctica. Polar Biol : Moreno J, DeLeon A, Fargallo JA, Moreno E (998) Breeding time, health and immune response in the chinstrap penguin Pygoscelis antarctica. Oecologia 5: 9 Norman FI, McFarlane RA, Ward SJ (99) Carcasses of Adèlie penguins as a food source for south polar skuas: some preliminary observations. Wilson Bull 6:6 Norris K, Evans MR () Ecological immunology: life history trade-offs and immune defense in birds. Behav Ecol :9 6 Penteriani V, Viñuela J, Belliure J, Bustamante J, Ferrer M () Causal and functional correlates of brood amalgamation in the chinstrap penguin Pygoscelis antarctica: parental decision and adult aggressiveness. Polar Biol 6:58 5 Pole A, Gordon IJ, Gorman ML () African wild dogs test the survival of the fittest paradigm. Proc R Soc Lond B 7:57 (Suppl) Quinn JL, Cresswell W () Predator hunting behaviour and prey vulnerability. J Anim Ecol 7: 5 Quinn JL, Cresswell W (5) Personality, anti-predation behaviour and behavioural plasticity in the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs. Behaviour :77 Roberts G (996) Why individual vigilance declines as group size increases? Anim Behav 5:77 86 Smith ME, Belk MC () Risk assessment in western mosquitofish (Gambusia affinis): do multiple cues have additive effects? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 5: 7 Smith KG, Hunt JL () On the use of spleen mass as a measure of avian immune system strength. Oecologia 8:8 Smits JE, Bortolotti GR, Tella JL (999) Simplifying the phytohaemagglutinin skin-testing technique in studies of avian immunocompetence. Funct Ecol : Stankowich T, Blumstein DT (5) Fear in animals: a metaanalysis and review of risk assessment. Proc R Soc Lond B 7: 67 6 Tella JL, Forero MG, Bertellotti M, Donázar JA, Blanco G, Ceballos O () Offspring body condition and immunocompetence are negatively affected by high breeding densities in a colonial sea bird: a multiscale approach. Proc R Soc Lond B 68: 7 Temple SA (986) Do predators always capture substandard individuals disproportionately from prey populations? Ecology 68: Van Heezik Y, Seddon PJ (99) Effect of human disturbance on beach groups of jackass penguins. S Afr J Wildl Res :89 9 Vega-Redondo F, Hasson O (99) A game-theoretic model of predator prey signaling. J Theor Biol 6:9 9 Viñuela J, Amat JA, Ferrer M (995) Nest defence of nesting chinstrap penguins (Pygoscelis antarctica) against intruders. Ethology 99: Viñuela J, Moreno J, Carrascal LM, Sanz JJ, Amat JA, Ferrer M, Belliure J, Cuervo JJ (996) The effect of hatching date on parental care, chick growth, and chick mortality in the chinstrap penguin Pygoscelis antarctica. J Zool :5 58 Williams TD (995) The penguins. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK Ydenberg RC, Dill LM (986) The economics of fleeing from predators. Adv Stud Behav 6:9 9 Yorio P, Boersma PD (99) The effects of human disturbance on Magellanic penguin Spheniscus magellanicus behaviour and breeding success. Bird Conserv Int :6 7 Young E (99) Skua and penguin. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK

Phenotypic selection on morphology at independence in the Chinstrap penguin Pygoscelis antarctica

Phenotypic selection on morphology at independence in the Chinstrap penguin Pygoscelis antarctica Phenotypic selection on morphology at independence in the Chinstrap penguin Pygoscelis antarctica J. MORENO, A. BARBOSA, A. DE LEOÂ N & J. A. FARGALLO Museo Nacional de Ciencias Naturales-CSIC, J. Gutierrez

More information

King penguin brooding and defending a sub-antarctic skua chick

King penguin brooding and defending a sub-antarctic skua chick King penguin brooding and defending a sub-antarctic skua chick W. Chris Oosthuizen 1 and P. J. Nico de Bruyn 1 (1) Department of Zoology and Entomology, Mammal Research Institute, University of Pretoria,

More information

Uncertainty about future predation risk modulates monitoring behavior from refuges in lizards

Uncertainty about future predation risk modulates monitoring behavior from refuges in lizards Behavioral Ecology doi:10.1093/beheco/arq065 Advance Access publication 13 January 2011 Original Article Uncertainty about future predation risk modulates monitoring behavior from refuges in lizards Vicente

More information

THE EVOLUTION OF BEGGING

THE EVOLUTION OF BEGGING THE EVOLUTION OF BEGGING COMPETITION, COOPERATION AND COMMUNICATION EDITED BY Jonathan Wright School of Biological Sciences, University of Wales, Bangor, U.K. AND Marty L. Leonard Department of Biology,

More information

Causes and Benefits of Chick Aggregations in Penguins

Causes and Benefits of Chick Aggregations in Penguins Causes and Benefits of Chick Aggregations in Penguins Author(s): David Wilson Source: The Auk, 126(3):688-693. Published By: The American Ornithologists' Union URL: http://www.bioone.org/doi/full/10.1525/auk.2009.9709

More information

Hatching asynchrony and brood reduction influence immune response in Common Kestrel Falco tinnunculus nestlings

Hatching asynchrony and brood reduction influence immune response in Common Kestrel Falco tinnunculus nestlings Ibis (2011), 153, 601 610 Hatching asynchrony and brood reduction influence immune response in Common Kestrel Falco tinnunculus nestlings JESÚS MARTÍNEZ-PADILLA 1,2 * & JAVIER VIÑUELA 3 1 Department of

More information

Polar Biol (1994) 14: Springer-Verlag 1994

Polar Biol (1994) 14: Springer-Verlag 1994 Polar Biol (1994) 14: 21-30 9 Springer-Verlag 1994 J. Moreno - L. M. Carrascal " J. J. Sanz J. A. Amat. J. J. Cuervo Hatching asynchrony, sibling hierarchies and brood reduction in the Chinstrap penguin

More information

Animal Behaviour 78 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Animal Behaviour. journal homepage:

Animal Behaviour 78 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Animal Behaviour. journal homepage: Animal Behaviour 78 (2009) 1011 1018 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Animal Behaviour journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/anbehav When to run from an ambush predator: balancing crypsis

More information

Environmental and genetic variation in T-cell-mediated immune response of fledgling American kestrels

Environmental and genetic variation in T-cell-mediated immune response of fledgling American kestrels Oecologia (2000) 123:453 459 Springer-Verlag 2000 José L. Tella Gary R. Bortolotti Manuela G. Forero Russell D. Dawson Environmental and genetic variation in T-cell-mediated immune response of fledgling

More information

Perceived risk of ectoparasitism reduces primary reproductive investment in tree swallows Tachycineta bicolor

Perceived risk of ectoparasitism reduces primary reproductive investment in tree swallows Tachycineta bicolor RESEARCH LETTERS Research letters are short papers (preferably 55 printed pages, about 4000 words), ideally presenting new and exciting results. Letters will be given priority, whenever possible, in the

More information

CHINSTRAP PENGUIN (PYGOSCELI$ ANTARCTICA): A FIELD EXPERIMENT

CHINSTRAP PENGUIN (PYGOSCELI$ ANTARCTICA): A FIELD EXPERIMENT The Auk 114(1):47-54, 1997 THE EFFECTS OF HATCHING DATE AND PARENTAL QUALITY ON CHICK GROWTH AND CRECHING AGE IN THE CHINSTRAP PENGUIN (PYGOSCELI$ ANTARCTICA): A FIELD EXPERIMENT JUAN MORENO, x'3 ANDRgS

More information

Effects of early incubation constancy on embryonic development: An experimental study in the herring gull Larus argentatus

Effects of early incubation constancy on embryonic development: An experimental study in the herring gull Larus argentatus Journal of Thermal Biology 31 (2006) 416 421 www.elsevier.com/locate/jtherbio Effects of early incubation constancy on embryonic development: An experimental study in the herring gull Larus argentatus

More information

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns Demography and Populations Survivorship Demography is the study of fecundity and survival Four critical variables Age of first breeding Number of young fledged each year Juvenile survival Adult survival

More information

The effect of testosterone injections on aggression and begging behaviour of black headed gull chicks (Larus ridibundus)

The effect of testosterone injections on aggression and begging behaviour of black headed gull chicks (Larus ridibundus) The effect of testosterone injections on aggression and begging behaviour of black headed gull chicks (Larus ridibundus) Abstract L.M. van Zomeren april 2009 supervised by Giuseppe Boncoraglio and Ton

More information

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition Proceedings of The National Conference on Undergraduate Research (NCUR) 2003 University of Utah, Salt Lake City, Utah March 13-15, 2003 Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris):

More information

Aggressiveness in king penguins in relation to reproductive status and territory location

Aggressiveness in king penguins in relation to reproductive status and territory location ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR,, 59, 813 821 doi:1.16/anbe.1999.1384, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Aggressiveness in king penguins in relation to reproductive status and territory location STEEVE

More information

Growth and Development. Embryonic development 2/22/2018. Timing of hatching. Hatching. Young birds and their parents

Growth and Development. Embryonic development 2/22/2018. Timing of hatching. Hatching. Young birds and their parents Growth and Development Young birds and their parents Embryonic development From fertilization to hatching, the embryo undergoes sequence of 42 distinct developmental stages The first 33 stages vary little

More information

Nest size in monogamous passerines has recently been hypothesized

Nest size in monogamous passerines has recently been hypothesized Behavioral Ecology Vol. 12 No. 3: 301 307 Nest size affects clutch size and the start of incubation in magpies: an experimental study Juan José Soler, a Liesbeth de Neve, b Juan Gabriel Martínez, b and

More information

Manipulating rearing conditions reveals developmental sensitivity in the smaller sex of a passerine bird, the European starling Sturnus vulgaris

Manipulating rearing conditions reveals developmental sensitivity in the smaller sex of a passerine bird, the European starling Sturnus vulgaris J. Avian Biol. 38: 612618, 2007 doi: 10.1111/j.2007.0908-8857.04082.x # 2007 The Authors. J. Compilation # 2007 J. Avian Biol. Received 28 September 2006, accepted 18 December 2006 Manipulating rearing

More information

STATE-DEPENDENT AND RISK-SENSITIVE ESCAPE DECISIONS IN A FOSSORIAL REPTILE, THE AMPHISBAENIAN BLANUS CINEREUS

STATE-DEPENDENT AND RISK-SENSITIVE ESCAPE DECISIONS IN A FOSSORIAL REPTILE, THE AMPHISBAENIAN BLANUS CINEREUS HERPETOLOGICAL JOURNAL, Vol. 10, pp. 27-32 (2000) STATE-DEPENDENT AND RISK-SENSITIVE ESCAPE DECISIONS IN A FOSSORIAL REPTILE, THE AMPHISBAENIAN BLANUS CINEREUS JOSE MARTIN, PILAR LOPEZ AND ANDRES BARBOSA

More information

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia) Luke Campillo and Aaron Claus IBS Animal Behavior Prof. Wisenden 6/25/2009 Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia) Abstract: The Song Sparrow

More information

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis I. P. JOHNSON and R. M. SIBLY Fourteen individually marked pairs o f Canada Geese were observedfrom January to April on their feeding grounds

More information

University of Groningen

University of Groningen University of Groningen No sexual differences in embryonic period in jackdaws Corvus monedula and black-headed gulls Larus ridibundus Salomons, Henri; Mueller, Wendt; Dijkstra, C; Eising, Corine; Verhulst,

More information

The effects of environmental and individual quality on reproductive performance Amininasab, Seyed Mehdi

The effects of environmental and individual quality on reproductive performance Amininasab, Seyed Mehdi University of Groningen The effects of environmental and individual quality on reproductive performance Amininasab, Seyed Mehdi IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's

More information

Juvenile Collared Lizards Adjust Tail Display Frequency in Response to Variable Predatory Threat

Juvenile Collared Lizards Adjust Tail Display Frequency in Response to Variable Predatory Threat Ethology Juvenile Collared Lizards Adjust Tail Display Frequency in Response to Variable Predatory Threat Joshua R. York* & Troy A. Baird * Department of Biology, University of Oklahoma, Norman, OK, USA

More information

1. Adélie Penguins can mate for life or at least try to find the same mate every year.

1. Adélie Penguins can mate for life or at least try to find the same mate every year. Banding Did You Know? 1. Adélie Penguins can mate for life or at least try to find the same mate every year. 2. Some Adélie Penguin colonies are increasing in size at a rate that cannot be due to just

More information

THE BEGGING BEHAVIOR OF NESTLING EASTERN SCREECH-OWLS

THE BEGGING BEHAVIOR OF NESTLING EASTERN SCREECH-OWLS Wilson Bulletin, 110(l), 1998, pp. 86-92 THE BEGGING BEHAVIOR OF NESTLING EASTERN SCREECH-OWLS STEPHEN H. HOFSTETTER AND GARY RITCHISON J ABSTRACT-The behavior of adults and nestlings at nine Eastern Screech-owl

More information

AVIAN PREDATION AT A SOUTHERN ROCKHOPPER PENGUIN COLONY ON STATEN ISLAND, ARGENTINA. Marcela Liljesthröm

AVIAN PREDATION AT A SOUTHERN ROCKHOPPER PENGUIN COLONY ON STATEN ISLAND, ARGENTINA. Marcela Liljesthröm AVIAN PREDATION AT A SOUTHERN ROCKHOPPER PENGUIN COLONY ON STATEN ISLAND, ARGENTINA Marcela Liljesthröm A Thesis Submitted to the University North Carolina Wilmington in Partial Fulfillment Of the Requirements

More information

Demography and breeding success of Falklands skua at Sea Lion Island, Falkland Islands

Demography and breeding success of Falklands skua at Sea Lion Island, Falkland Islands Filippo Galimberti and Simona Sanvito Elephant Seal Research Group Demography and breeding success of Falklands skua at Sea Lion Island, Falkland Islands Field work report - Update 2018/2019 25/03/2019

More information

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153) i Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN 978-1-927194-58-4, page 153) Activity 9: Intraspecific relationships extra questions

More information

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp SHORT COMMUNICATIONS 693 Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp. 693-697 Conspecific aggression in a Wood Stork colony in Georgia.-The probability of interactions among conspecifics, including aggression, is

More information

3. Chicks weigh 86 grams when they hatch and gain 100 grams a day until they are about 50 days old when they are ready to take care of itself.

3. Chicks weigh 86 grams when they hatch and gain 100 grams a day until they are about 50 days old when they are ready to take care of itself. Did You Know? Direct Observation 1. The average nest has 200 rocks. 2. It takes between 30-35 days for an Adélie Penguin egg to hatch. 3. Chicks weigh 86 grams when they hatch and gain 100 grams a day

More information

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production May 2013 Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production Michael Longley, Global Technical Transfer Manager Summary Introduction Chick numbers are most often reduced during the period

More information

Flexibility in antipredatory behavior allows wall lizards to cope with multiple types of predators

Flexibility in antipredatory behavior allows wall lizards to cope with multiple types of predators Ann. Zool. Fennici 42: 109 121 ISSN 0003-455X Helsinki 26 April 2005 Finnish Zoological and Botanical Publishing Board 2005 Flexibility in antipredatory behavior allows wall lizards to cope with multiple

More information

Aspects of the breeding biology of the southern rockhopper penguin Eudyptes c. chrysocome and new consideration on the intrinsic capacity of the A-egg

Aspects of the breeding biology of the southern rockhopper penguin Eudyptes c. chrysocome and new consideration on the intrinsic capacity of the A-egg Polar Biol (2008) 31:925 932 DOI 10.1007/s00300-008-0431-2 ORIGINAL PAPER Aspects of the breeding biology of the southern rockhopper penguin Eudyptes c. chrysocome and new consideration on the intrinsic

More information

Life-history theories suggest that present reproductive effort

Life-history theories suggest that present reproductive effort Behavioral Ecology Vol. 13 No. 4: 575 579 Impaired flight ability a cost of reproduction in female blue tits Cecilia Kullberg, David C. Houston, and Neil B. Metcalfe Ornithology Group, Division of Environmental

More information

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production Michael Longley, Global Technical Transfer Manager May 2013 SUMMARY Introduction Chick numbers are most often reduced during the period

More information

Below, we present the methods used to address these objectives, our preliminary results and next steps in this multi-year project.

Below, we present the methods used to address these objectives, our preliminary results and next steps in this multi-year project. Background Final Report to the Nova Scotia Habitat Conservation Fund: Determining the role of food availability on swallow population declines Project Supervisor: Tara Imlay, tara.imlay@dal.ca In the past

More information

Supplementary Fig. 1: Comparison of chase parameters for focal pack (a-f, n=1119) and for 4 dogs from 3 other packs (g-m, n=107).

Supplementary Fig. 1: Comparison of chase parameters for focal pack (a-f, n=1119) and for 4 dogs from 3 other packs (g-m, n=107). Supplementary Fig. 1: Comparison of chase parameters for focal pack (a-f, n=1119) and for 4 dogs from 3 other packs (g-m, n=107). (a,g) Maximum stride speed, (b,h) maximum tangential acceleration, (c,i)

More information

The role of visual cues in learning escape behaviour in the little brown skink (Scincella lateralis)

The role of visual cues in learning escape behaviour in the little brown skink (Scincella lateralis) Behaviour 151 (2014) 2015 2028 brill.com/beh The role of visual cues in learning escape behaviour in the little brown skink (Scincella lateralis) Mark A. Paulissen Department of Natural Sciences, Northeastern

More information

769 q 2005 The Royal Society

769 q 2005 The Royal Society 272, 769 773 doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.3039 Published online 7 April 2005 Life-history variation of a neotropical thrush challenges food limitation theory Valentina Ferretti 1,2, *,, Paulo E. Llambías 1,2,

More information

How Does Photostimulation Age Alter the Interaction Between Body Size and a Bonus Feeding Program During Sexual Maturation?

How Does Photostimulation Age Alter the Interaction Between Body Size and a Bonus Feeding Program During Sexual Maturation? 16 How Does Photostimulation Age Alter the Interaction Between Body Size and a Bonus Feeding Program During Sexual Maturation? R A Renema*, F E Robinson*, and J A Proudman** *Alberta Poultry Research Centre,

More information

EFFECTS OF FOOD SUPPLEMENTATION AND HABITAT SELECTION ON TIMING OF LESSER KESTREL BREEDING

EFFECTS OF FOOD SUPPLEMENTATION AND HABITAT SELECTION ON TIMING OF LESSER KESTREL BREEDING Notes Ecology, 83(3), 2002, pp. 873 877 2002 by the Ecological Society of America EFFECTS OF FOOD SUPPLEMENTATION AND HABITAT SELECTION ON TIMING OF LESSER KESTREL BREEDING JOSÉ MIGUEL APARICIO 1 AND RAÚL

More information

EXERCISE 14 Marine Birds at Sea World Name

EXERCISE 14 Marine Birds at Sea World Name EXERCISE 14 Marine Birds at Sea World Name Section Polar and Equatorial Penguins Penguins Penguins are flightless birds that are mainly concentrated in the Southern Hemisphere. They were first discovered

More information

University of Groningen. Offspring fitness and individual optimization of clutch size Both, C; Tinbergen, Joost; Noordwijk, Arie J.

University of Groningen. Offspring fitness and individual optimization of clutch size Both, C; Tinbergen, Joost; Noordwijk, Arie J. University of Groningen Offspring fitness and individual optimization of clutch size Both, C; Tinbergen, Joost; Noordwijk, Arie J. van Published in: Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B,

More information

Presence of Males Within Laying Hens Affects Tonic Immobility Response and Sociality

Presence of Males Within Laying Hens Affects Tonic Immobility Response and Sociality International Journal of Poultry Science 9 (12): 1087-1091, 2010 ISSN 1682-8356 Asian Network for Scientific Information, 2010 Presence of Males Within Laying Hens Affects Tonic Immobility Response and

More information

Awide diversity of cues in the animal kingdom has evolved. Liesbeth De Neve, a Juan José Soler, b Manuel Soler, a and Tomás Pérez-Contreras b

Awide diversity of cues in the animal kingdom has evolved. Liesbeth De Neve, a Juan José Soler, b Manuel Soler, a and Tomás Pérez-Contreras b Behavioral Ecology Vol. 15 No. 6: 1031 1036 doi:10.1093/beheco/arh074 Advance Access publication on July 7, 2004 Nest size predicts the effect of food supplementation to magpie nestlings on their immunocompetence:

More information

DO DIFFERENT CLUTCH SIZES OF THE TREE SWALLOW (Tachycineta bicolor)

DO DIFFERENT CLUTCH SIZES OF THE TREE SWALLOW (Tachycineta bicolor) DO DIFFERENT CLUTCH SIZES OF THE TREE SWALLOW (Tachycineta bicolor) HAVE VARYING FLEDGLING SUCCESS? Cassandra Walker August 25 th, 2017 Abstract Tachycineta bicolor (Tree Swallow) were surveyed over a

More information

Short-term and Long-term Consequences of Predator Avoidance by Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor)

Short-term and Long-term Consequences of Predator Avoidance by Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) Made in United States of America Reprinted from THE AUK Vol. 108, No.3, July 1991 Copyright @ 1991 by The American Ornithologists' Union Short-term and Long-term Consequences of Predator Avoidance by Tree

More information

Assessing the effectiveness of temporarily excluding tourists from a Yellow-Eyed Penguin (Megadyptes antipodes) breeding beach

Assessing the effectiveness of temporarily excluding tourists from a Yellow-Eyed Penguin (Megadyptes antipodes) breeding beach Assessing the effectiveness of temporarily excluding tourists from a Yellow-Eyed Penguin (Megadyptes antipodes) breeding beach Matt Conley A report submitted in partial fulfilment of the Post-graduate

More information

Predation risk, cover type, and group size in European rabbits in Donana (SW Spain)

Predation risk, cover type, and group size in European rabbits in Donana (SW Spain) Acta Theriologica 42 (2): 225-230, 1997. PL ISSN 0001-7051 Predation risk, cover type, and group size in European rabbits in Donana (SW Spain) Rafael VILLAFUERTE and Sacramento MORENO Villafuerte R. and

More information

Does begging affect growth in nestling tree swallows, Tachycineta bicolor?

Does begging affect growth in nestling tree swallows, Tachycineta bicolor? Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2003) 54:573 577 DOI 10.1007/s00265-003-0668-2 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Marty L. Leonard Andrew G. Horn Jackie Porter Does begging affect growth in nestling tree swallows, Tachycineta bicolor?

More information

AGE AT FIRST BREEDING AND CHANGE IN PLUMAGE OF KELP GULLS LARUS DOMINICANUS IN SOUTH AFRICA. R. J. M. CRAWFORD*, B. M. DYER* and L.

AGE AT FIRST BREEDING AND CHANGE IN PLUMAGE OF KELP GULLS LARUS DOMINICANUS IN SOUTH AFRICA. R. J. M. CRAWFORD*, B. M. DYER* and L. S. Afr. J. mar. Sci. 22: 27 32 2000 27 AGE AT FIRST BREEDING AND CHANGE IN PLUMAGE OF KELP GULLS LARUS DOMINICANUS IN SOUTH AFRICA R. J. M. CRAWFORD*, B. M. DYER* and L. UPFOLD* In South Africa, kelp gulls

More information

DO BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS?

DO BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS? Wilson Bull., 0(4), 989, pp. 599605 DO BROWNHEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF REDWINGED BLACKBIRDS? GORDON H. ORTANS, EIVIN RDSKAPT, AND LES D. BELETSKY AssrnAcr.We tested the hypothesis

More information

Fitness cost of incubation in great tits (Parus major) is related to clutch size de Heij, Maaike E.; van den Hout, Piet J.

Fitness cost of incubation in great tits (Parus major) is related to clutch size de Heij, Maaike E.; van den Hout, Piet J. University of Groningen Fitness cost of incubation in great tits (Parus major) is related to clutch size de Heij, Maaike E.; van den Hout, Piet J.; Tinbergen, Joost Published in: Proceedings of the Royal

More information

Ornithological Observations

Ornithological Observations Ornithological Observations An electronic journal published by BirdLife South Africa and the Animal Demography Unit at the University of Cape Town Ornithological Observations accepts papers containing

More information

GULLS (LARUS ARGENTATUS)

GULLS (LARUS ARGENTATUS) TERRITORY SIZE DIFFERENCES IN RELATION TO REPRODUCTIVE STAGE AND TYPE OF INTRUDER IN HERRING GULLS (LARUS ARGENTATUS) JOANNA BURGER Department of Biology, Livingston College, Rutgers University, New Brunswick,

More information

A future cost of misdirected parental care for brood parasitic young?

A future cost of misdirected parental care for brood parasitic young? Folia Zool. 55(4): 367 374 (2006) A future cost of misdirected parental care for brood parasitic young? Mark E. HAUBER School of Biological Sciences, University of Auckland, Auckland, PB 92019, New Zealand;

More information

ethology Ethology RESEARCH PAPER

ethology Ethology RESEARCH PAPER international journal of behavioural biology ethology Ethology RESEARCH PAPER The Effect of Human Presence and Human Activity on Risk Assessment and Flight Initiation Distance in Skinks Marilyn M. McGowan,

More information

ANALYSIS OF GROWTH OF THE RED-TAILED HAWK 1

ANALYSIS OF GROWTH OF THE RED-TAILED HAWK 1 OhioJ. Sci. DEVONIAN ICROPHYTOPLANKTON 13 Copyright 1983 Ohio Acad. Sci. OO3O-O95O/83/OOO1-OO13 $2.00/0 ANALYSIS O GROWTH O THE RED-TAILED HAWK 1 ARK A. SPRINGER 2 and DAVID R. OSBORNE, Department of Zoology,

More information

BREEDING ECOLOGY OF THE LITTLE TERN, STERNA ALBIFRONS PALLAS, 1764 IN SINGAPORE

BREEDING ECOLOGY OF THE LITTLE TERN, STERNA ALBIFRONS PALLAS, 1764 IN SINGAPORE NATURE IN SINGAPORE 2008 1: 69 73 Date of Publication: 10 September 2008 National University of Singapore BREEDING ECOLOGY OF THE LITTLE TERN, STERNA ALBIFRONS PALLAS, 1764 IN SINGAPORE J. W. K. Cheah*

More information

State-dependent parental care in the Antarctic petrel: responses to manipulated chick age during early chick rearing

State-dependent parental care in the Antarctic petrel: responses to manipulated chick age during early chick rearing OIKOS 106: 479/488, 2004 State-dependent parental care in the Antarctic petrel: responses to manipulated chick age during early chick rearing Øystein Varpe, Torkild Tveraa and Ivar Folstad Varpe, Ø., Tveraa,

More information

Egg size, offspring sex and hatching asynchrony in zebra finches Taeniopygia guttata

Egg size, offspring sex and hatching asynchrony in zebra finches Taeniopygia guttata JOURNAL OF AVIAN BIOLOGY 36: 12/17, 2005 Egg size, offspring sex and hatching asynchrony in zebra finches Taeniopygia guttata Joanna Rutkowska and Mariusz Cichoń Rutkowska, J. and Cichoń, M. 2005. Egg

More information

MITOCW MIT9_20F13_lec17.mp3

MITOCW MIT9_20F13_lec17.mp3 MITOCW MIT9_20F13_lec17.mp3 The following content is provided under a Creative Commons license. Your support will help MIT OpenCourseWare continue to offer high quality educational resources for free.

More information

Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey

Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring - 2011 Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey October 2011 1 Cover photograph: Egyptian vulture landing in Beypazarı dump site, photographed

More information

Is asynchronous hatching adaptive in herring gulls (Larus argentatus)?

Is asynchronous hatching adaptive in herring gulls (Larus argentatus)? Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2000) 47:304 311 Springer-Verlag 2000 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Lars Hillström Mikael Kilpi Kai Lindström Is asynchronous hatching adaptive in herring gulls (Larus argentatus)? Received: 14

More information

Behavioural responses to ectoparasites: time-budget adjustments and what matters to Blue Tits Parus caeruleus infested by fleas

Behavioural responses to ectoparasites: time-budget adjustments and what matters to Blue Tits Parus caeruleus infested by fleas Ibis (2002), 144, 461 469 Blackwell Science Ltd Behavioural responses to ectoparasites: time-budget adjustments and what matters to Blue Tits Parus caeruleus infested by fleas FRÉDÉRIC TRIPET,* MARKUS

More information

Introduction. Lizards: very diverse colour patterns intra- and interspecific differences in colour

Introduction. Lizards: very diverse colour patterns intra- and interspecific differences in colour Jessica Vroonen Introduction Lizards: very diverse colour patterns intra- and interspecific differences in colour Introduction Lizards intra- and interspecific differences in colour Introduction Lizards

More information

Population dynamics of small game. Pekka Helle Natural Resources Institute Finland Luke Oulu

Population dynamics of small game. Pekka Helle Natural Resources Institute Finland Luke Oulu Population dynamics of small game Pekka Helle Natural Resources Institute Finland Luke Oulu Populations tend to vary in size temporally, some species show more variation than others Depends on degree of

More information

doi: /

doi: / doi: 10.2326/1347-0558-7.2.117 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Methods for correcting plumage color fading in the Barn Swallow Masaru HASEGAWA 1,#, Emi ARAI 2, Mamoru WATANABE 1 and Masahiko NAKAMURA 2 1 Graduate School

More information

Selection for Egg Mass in the Domestic Fowl. 1. Response to Selection

Selection for Egg Mass in the Domestic Fowl. 1. Response to Selection Selection for Egg Mass in the Domestic Fowl. 1. Response to Selection H. L. MARKS US Department of Agriculture, Science & Education Administration, Agricultural Research, uthern Regional Poultry Breeding

More information

Habitat-specific effects of a food supplementation experiment on immunocompetence in Eurasian Magpie Pica pica nestlings

Habitat-specific effects of a food supplementation experiment on immunocompetence in Eurasian Magpie Pica pica nestlings Ibis (2007), 149, 763 773 Blackwell Publishing Ltd Habitat-specific effects of a food supplementation experiment on immunocompetence in Eurasian Magpie Pica pica nestlings LIESBETH DE NEVE, 1 * JUAN J.

More information

What is the date at which most chicks would have been expected to fledge?

What is the date at which most chicks would have been expected to fledge? CURLEW FAQs FACTS AND FIGURES AND ADVICE FOR THOSE WANTING TO HELP SUPPORT NESTING CURLEW ON THEIR LAND The Eurasian Curlew or, Numenius arquata, spends much of the year on coasts or estuaries, but migrates

More information

The influence of hatching order on the thermoregulatory behaviour of barn owl Tyto alba nestlings

The influence of hatching order on the thermoregulatory behaviour of barn owl Tyto alba nestlings Avian Science Vol. 2 No. 3: 167-173 (2002) ISSN 1424-8743 167 The influence of hatching order on the thermoregulatory behaviour of barn owl Tyto alba nestlings Joël M. Durant The behavioural responses

More information

Like mother, like daughter: inheritance of nest-site

Like mother, like daughter: inheritance of nest-site Like mother, like daughter: inheritance of nest-site location in snakes Gregory P. Brown and Richard Shine* School of Biological Sciences A0, University of Sydney, NSW 00, Australia *Author for correspondence

More information

AS91603 Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants & animals to their external environment

AS91603 Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants & animals to their external environment AS91603 Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants & animals to their external environment Animal behaviour (2015, 1) Some animals display innate behaviours. As green bottle fly maggots (Phaenicia

More information

The effects of diet upon pupal development and cocoon formation by the cat flea (Siphonaptera: Pulicidae)

The effects of diet upon pupal development and cocoon formation by the cat flea (Siphonaptera: Pulicidae) June, 2002 Journal of Vector Ecology 39 The effects of diet upon pupal development and cocoon formation by the cat flea (Siphonaptera: Pulicidae) W. Lawrence and L. D. Foil Department of Entomology, Louisiana

More information

Bear Awareness Training

Bear Awareness Training Bear Awareness Training Please review the following presentation. In order to move the presentation forward or back, simply click on your mouse or use your scroll wheel. If you have any questions on how

More information

Genetic parameters and breeding value stability estimated from a joint evaluation of purebred and crossbred sows for litter weight at weaning

Genetic parameters and breeding value stability estimated from a joint evaluation of purebred and crossbred sows for litter weight at weaning Acta Agraria Kaposváriensis (2015) Vol 19 No 1, 1-7. Kaposvári Egyetem, Agrár- és Környezettudományi Kar, Kaposvár Genetic parameters and breeding value stability estimated from a joint evaluation of purebred

More information

Genetics of temperament: What do we know about the back test?

Genetics of temperament: What do we know about the back test? Genetics of temperament: What do we know about the back test? Kim Bunter 1 and Richard Lansdowne 2 1 Animal Genetics and Breeding Unit, University of New England, Armidale, NSW, 2351. 2 Faculty of Veterinary

More information

Factors Influencing Local Recruitment in Tree Swallows, Tachycineta bicolor

Factors Influencing Local Recruitment in Tree Swallows, Tachycineta bicolor Grand Valley State University ScholarWorks@GVSU Honors Projects Undergraduate Research and Creative Practice 2013 Factors Influencing Local Recruitment in Tree Swallows, Tachycineta bicolor Danielle M.

More information

Animal Behavior: Biology 3401 Laboratory 4: Social behaviour of young domestic chickens

Animal Behavior: Biology 3401 Laboratory 4: Social behaviour of young domestic chickens 1 Introduction: Animal Behavior: Biology 3401 Laboratory 4: Social behaviour of young domestic chickens In many species, social interactions among siblings and (or) between siblings and their parents during

More information

clutch size and escape take-off speed in female zebra finches

clutch size and escape take-off speed in female zebra finches Ecology 2001 70, A hidden cost of reproduction: the trade-off between Blackwell Science, Ltd clutch size and escape take-off speed in female zebra finches JAKE S. VEASEY, DAVID C. HOUSTON and NEIL B. METCALFE

More information

The evolution of conspicuous begging has been a topic of

The evolution of conspicuous begging has been a topic of Behavioral Ecology Vol. 11 No. 2: 196 201 Brood size and begging intensity in nestling birds Marty L. Leonard, Andrew G. Horn, Alison Gozna, and Satya Ramen Department of Biology, Dalhousie University,

More information

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Territorial signals. Design rules for territorial signals. Why defend a territory? Bird song and territory defense

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Territorial signals. Design rules for territorial signals. Why defend a territory? Bird song and territory defense PSY 2364 Animal Communication Territorial signals Territory in ecology, any area defended by an organism or a group of similar organisms for such purposes as mating, nesting, roosting, or feeding. Home

More information

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories Chapters 12 16 Many details in book, esp know: Chpt 12 pg 338-345, 359-365 Chpt 13 pg 367-373, 377-381, 385-391 Table 13-1 Chpt 14 pg 420-422, 427-430 Chpt 15 pg 431-438,

More information

Quantifying density dependence in a bird population using human disturbance

Quantifying density dependence in a bird population using human disturbance Oecologia (2007) 153:49 56 DOI 10.1007/s00442-007-0716-0 POPULATION ECOLOGY Quantifying density dependence in a bird population using human disturbance John W. Mallord Æ Paul M. Dolman Æ Andy Brown Æ William

More information

Male parental care and monogamy in snow buntings

Male parental care and monogamy in snow buntings Behav Ecol Sociobiol (1987) 20:377-382 Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 9 Springer-Verlag 1987 Male parental care and monogamy in snow buntings Bruce E. Lyon*, Robert D. Montgomerie, and Linda D. Hamilton*

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

08 alberts part2 7/23/03 9:10 AM Page 95 PART TWO. Behavior and Ecology

08 alberts part2 7/23/03 9:10 AM Page 95 PART TWO. Behavior and Ecology 08 alberts part2 7/23/03 9:10 AM Page 95 PART TWO Behavior and Ecology 08 alberts part2 7/23/03 9:10 AM Page 96 08 alberts part2 7/23/03 9:10 AM Page 97 Introduction Emília P. Martins Iguanas have long

More information

Tristan Darwin Project. Monitoring Guide. A Guide to Monitoring Albatross, Penguin and Seal Plots on Tristan and Nightingale

Tristan Darwin Project. Monitoring Guide. A Guide to Monitoring Albatross, Penguin and Seal Plots on Tristan and Nightingale Tristan Darwin Project Monitoring Guide A Guide to Monitoring Albatross, Penguin and Seal Plots on Tristan and Nightingale Atlantic Yellow-nosed albatross Biology The yellow-nosed albatross or molly lays

More information

Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) 2010 Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey

Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) 2010 Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) 2010 Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey December 2010 1 Cover photograph: Egyptian vulture taking off in Beypazarı dump site, photographed by Kadir Dabak.

More information

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS Nov., 1965 505 BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS Lack ( 1954; 40-41) has pointed out that in species of birds which have asynchronous hatching, brood size may be adjusted

More information

Do climatic conditions affect host and parasite phenotypes differentially? A case study of magpies and great spotted cuckoos

Do climatic conditions affect host and parasite phenotypes differentially? A case study of magpies and great spotted cuckoos Oecologia (2014) 174:327 338 DOI 10.1007/s00442-013-2772-y Physiological ecology - Original research Do climatic conditions affect host and parasite phenotypes differentially? A case study of magpies and

More information

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl) Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl) Family: Strigidae (Typical Owls) Order: Strigiformes (Owls) Class: Aves (Birds) Fig. 1. Mottled owl, Ciccaba virgata. [http://www.owling.com/mottled13.htm, downloaded 12 November

More information

Feeding the Commercial Egg-Type Replacement Pullet 1

Feeding the Commercial Egg-Type Replacement Pullet 1 PS48 Feeding the Commercial Egg-Type Replacement Pullet 1 Richard D. Miles and Jacqueline P. Jacob 2 TODAY'S PULLET Advances in genetic selection make today's pullets quite different from those of only

More information

THE EFFECT OF MAGPIE BREEDING DENSITY AND SYNCHRONY ON BROOD PARASITISM BY GREAT SPOTTED CUCKOOS

THE EFFECT OF MAGPIE BREEDING DENSITY AND SYNCHRONY ON BROOD PARASITISM BY GREAT SPOTTED CUCKOOS The Condor 98:272-278 0 The Cooper Ornithological Society 1996 THE EFFECT OF MAGPIE BREEDING DENSITY AND SYNCHRONY ON BROOD PARASITISM BY GREAT SPOTTED CUCKOOS J. G. MARTINEZ,~ M. SOLER AND J. J. SOLER

More information

BLACK OYSTERCATCHER NEST MONITORING PROTOCOL

BLACK OYSTERCATCHER NEST MONITORING PROTOCOL BLACK OYSTERCATCHER NEST MONITORING PROTOCOL In addition to the mid-late May population survey (see Black Oystercatcher abundance survey protocol) we will attempt to continue monitoring at least 25 nests

More information

SLOW DOWN, LOVE WIZARD. HERE S WHAT YOU NEED TO KNOW ABOUT THE HORNED LIZARD.

SLOW DOWN, LOVE WIZARD. HERE S WHAT YOU NEED TO KNOW ABOUT THE HORNED LIZARD. SLOW DOWN, LOVE WIZARD. HERE S WHAT YOU NEED TO KNOW ABOUT THE HORNED LIZARD. Horned lizards predominately eat ants. In small doses the ants venom does not harm the lizard; however, a swarm can kill an

More information