The e ect of helpers on reproductive success in the laughing kookaburra

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "The e ect of helpers on reproductive success in the laughing kookaburra"

Transcription

1 Ecology 2000, 69, The e ect of helpers on reproductive success in the laughing kookaburra SARAH LEGGE Evolutionary Ecology Group, Botany and Zoology, Australian National University, Anu, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia Summary 1. In co-operatively breeding birds and mammals that live in family groups, helpers may gain indirect tness bene ts by increasing the number of breeding attempts in a season, by increasing the success of each nesting attempt, and by increasing the survivorship of related breeders. 2. The e ects of helpers were examined in an analysis of reproductive success in the laughing kookaburra (Dacelo novaeguineae), where a monogamously breeding pair is assisted by o spring of both sexes. 3. Helpers could not increase the number of attempts in a season, because kookaburras are single-brooded. 4. In a paired analysis controlling for pair and territory quality, increases in group size were not matched by increases in the number or weight of edglings in each breeding attempt, even though brood reduction was the major source of productivity loss. This concurs with previous results showing that group size does not a ect overall provisioning levels to broods, because all group members reduced their provisioning e ort as group size increased. 5. Because kookaburras prefer to reduce workloads during breeding rather than raise larger broods, the costs of caring for young must be high. Thus breeders with helpers may enjoy energetic savings and enhanced survivorship. 6. Helpers had di erent e ects on nest success depending on their sex. Whereas male helpers had a neutral e ect on edging success and edgling weight, additional female helpers reduced edging success. Females are relatively poor helpers, and other group members may not properly compensate for the female's `inadequate' help. 7. High-quality kookaburra pairs, or pairs on good territories, were more likely to be in larger groups because they produced more potential recruits for their groups than relatively `poor' performers. This was demonstrated by comparing the results of the paired comparisons with those of a correlational analysis that showed that in groups of two to six birds, group size was positively correlated with edging success and edgling weight (which a ects the probability of juvenile survival). 8. However, unusually large groups of seven or more birds su ered drastically reduced nest success because eggs were damaged during incubation. Key-words: co-operative breeding, costs of helping, king sher, kin selection, philopatry. Ecology (2000) 69, Introduction The search for the adaptive basis to co-operative breeding has generated a long list of potential bene- Correspondence: Sarah Legge (fax (Australia) ; Sarah.Legge@anu.edu.au). ts to both breeders and helpers (review by Cockburn 1998). Most co-operatively breeding birds live in groups of relatives (Brown 1987) and this has strongly encouraged the idea that kin selection (Hamilton 1964) is a major selective force in the evolution of co-operative breeding (Emlen 1997). Helpers could boost the production of non-descen-

2 715 S. Legge dent kin by increasing the breeder's productivity in each breeding attempt (Brown et al. 1982; Mumme 1992) or by increasing the number of breeding attempts in a season (Brown & Brown 1981; Innes & Johnston 1996). In addition, they could increase the survivorship and reproductive life span of breeders by reducing the latter's workload in each breeding attempt (Mumme, Koenig & Ratnieks 1989; Crick 1992). However, the evidence that helpers increase group productivity is inconsistent. Some correlational studies have shown strong positive e ects (Lewis 1981; Emlen & Wrege 1991; Heinsohn 1992) but others have shown that helpers have no discernible e ect on productivity (Gaston 1978a; Monadjem, Owen- Smith & Kemp 1995). More importantly, positive correlations may be driven by the confounding e ects of pair and territory quality (Brown 1987; Cockburn 1998). Enhanced reproductive performance by high quality, older or experienced birds, or pairs on premium territories, may result in larger groups. Continued high performance can then be wrongly attributed to the e ects of group size. Confounding e ects have been controlled for by monitoring productivity after experimental removal of helpers (Brown et al. 1982; Leonard, Horn & Eden 1989; Mumme 1992). Less disruptive approaches include manipulating the postulated source of mortality that helpers alleviate (Austad & Rabenold 1985; Boland, Heinsohn & Cockburn 1997) or comparing the productivity of the same groups at di erent group sizes (Green et al. 1995), or groups of the same size but with di erent levels of help (Magrath & Yezerinac 1997). In some of these studies helpers did indeed increase productivity through alloparental care (Mumme 1992; Boland et al. 1997), but in others they had no apparent e ect (Leonard et al. 1989; Magrath & Yezerinac 1997). Group living can have advantages for the reproduction or survival of individuals (Ekman, Bylin & TegelstroÈ m 1999) irrespective of whether group members are related or not. For example, groups may perform better in territorial contests (Craig 1984), be more e ective in predator detection (Woolfenden & Fitzpatrick 1990) or cope more e ectively with ecological constraints, such as thermoregulation in cold environments (Sydeman, Guntert & Balda 1988; Ligon & Ligon 1990). Groups may also be good forums for nding mates (Reyer, Dittami & Hall 1986; Sherley 1989). Young birds in particular may reap the rewards from a `supportive' environment, allowing them to learn foraging and breeding skills under the wing of extended parental care (Brown 1987; Heinsohn 1991). Even if they are secondarily evolved to some extent, parental facilitation and other bene ts of group living could be powerful selective forces maintaining co-operative breeding. The e ects of helpers on reproductive success are examined here in the laughing kookaburra (Dacelo novaeguineae, Hermann), a large terrestrial king- sher endemic to eastern Australia. Laughing kookaburras live in co-operatively breeding groups of two to eight birds. A socially monogamous and dominant pair is assisted by helpers of either sex that are o spring from previous breeding attempts (Legge & Cockburn 2000). All helpers participate with all aspects of clutch, nestling and edgling care, even developing brood patches (Legge, 2000a). They also vigorously help to defend the group territory, which is maintained all year (Parry 1973; Reyer & Schmidl 1988; S. Legge, unpublished data). Territorial inheritance, a common feature of other co-operative breeders, is rare or absent, and helpers gain extremely few direct mating bene ts (Legge & Cockburn 2000). Thus it is pertinent to ask whether helpers gain indirect tness bene ts by in uencing the production of non-descendent kin. As kookaburras are single-brooded, helpers cannot increase the number of nesting attempts in a season. However, brood reduction is common, therefore additional helpers may increase the success of each breeding attempt. In addition, their presence and contribution during breeding may enhance the survival and lifetime reproductive success of related breeders. Methods The study was carried out between December 1994 and February 1998 in 20 km 2 of eucalypt woodland near Canberra, south-east Australia (Mt Majura, Mt Ainslie, O'Connor ridge, Aranda Bushland and Black Mountain, including the Australian National Botanic Gardens). This period included years with very di erent weather conditions. The annual mean rainfall for 1988±98 was 655 mm (Bureau of Meteorology, Canberra, Australia) was characterized by the coldest winter in 150 years, followed by a severe drought during the breeding season was also a dry year (411 mm), but 1995 was a particularly wet year (801 mm). About 130 birds in 35 groups lived in the study area each year. Group territorial displays marked the position of each group's territory boundaries (Parry 1973; Reyer & Schmidl 1988). Birds were identi ed from coloured aluminium bands, patagial tags, or distinctive plumage markings that were retained between moults. Group composition and the relationships between birds in this population were known in detail from long-term monitoring and DNA ngerprinting (Legge & Cockburn 2000). Although both sexes help, male helpers predominate (68±82% in any one year) because females disperse at an earlier age. Helpers disperse to breeding positions, but never to helping positions in other groups. Dominant birds retain their status for many years, so in most groups all helpers were the o spring of

3 716 Reproductive success in kookaburras the dominant pair. However, step-families form through death and replacement of one of the breeders, usually the female, making it possible to compare the productivity of groups where the relatedness of helpers to the brood varies. The tenure of individuals within groups was monitored with weekly nest watches during nesting, then fortnightly censuses for 2 months after edging, and opportunistically throughout the rest of the year. Kookaburras were normally found sitting in old gum trees. The exact age of some dominant birds was known because they had been banded as edglings, but most dominants were assigned a minimum age based on the number of o spring helpers (known from DNA ngerprinting) in their group when rst encountered. Birds that immigrated into a dominant position during the study could be aged dichotomously as 1 year old or 2, based on tail feather shape. Birds were sexed using a molecular technique (Gri ths et al. 1998). The few uncaught birds (less than 15%) were assigned a sex based on behaviour (e.g. the mate of a known-sex bird) or plumage characteristics (Higgins 1999). Clutches were laid in the austral spring (September±December) in unprepared hollows 4±12 m from the ground, mainly in eucalypts (Eucalyptus rossii, R.T. Baker & H.G. Smith, E. mannifera, Mudie, E. blakleyii, Maiden, E. melliodora, A. Cunn ex Schauer). Hollows form as a result of termite action, branch fall and wind shear. Kookaburras normally lay three eggs (range 1±4) at 1±4 day intervals. Incubation begins before the clutch is complete and lasts 24±26 days. Eggs hatch asynchronously at 2- to 72-h intervals. Nestlings edge at about 35 days, and forage independently after a further 8 weeks (Parry 1973; S. Legge, unpublished data). Nests were usually found by ushing incubating birds, and reached using single-rope techniques. The laying date was estimated by candling eggs. Nests were visited every 1 or 2 days as required near the estimated hatching date until all eggs had hatched. Thereafter the progress of nests was checked weekly. Unhatched or missing eggs were noted, as were nestling disappearances. Fledging success and edging weight were taken as the number and weight of nestlings present on the nal nest visit at 32 days, when nestlings were also banded. In 16 group-years no clutch was found; although it was suspected that eggs were never laid, a clutch may have been initiated but quickly lost. These data were omitted from the analysis of clutch sizes and hatching success, but retained in the analysis of edging success. Failure to lay eggs is as relevant as egg or nestling loss in determining the number of young edged in a season, and the decision to lay or not may be in uenced by the presence of helpers. In 126 groupyears, ve groups had a second breeding attempt in a single season after the rst attempt failed, making 131 attempts available for analysis of edging success. Data on clutch size and hatching success were fewer (89 attempts in 84 group-years) because most nests from 1994 were found after hatching, some nests in later years were inaccessible, and because the 16 group-years with `no clutch' were excluded. Breeding in 43 of the 126 group-years (341%) was by unassisted pairs. The remainder involved groups with one to ve helpers. The median group size was three. ANALYSIS Using Genstat 5 (release 4.1; Genstat Committee 1993), a statistical modelling approach was used to consider environmental as well as demographic variables that might a ect reproductive success. The analysis was framed as three questions. First, what a ects hatching and edging success? Second, as an alternative to numbers of young, what a ects edgling weight? and third, what in uences edgling survival to 2 months, when it is nutritionally independent? In all these questions, the e ect of helpers was of particular interest, but other social as well as environmental variables were considered in the models. Generally, there was potential for dependency within the data, as groups were represented one to four times, and nests contained one to three edglings. To account for repeated sampling with an unbalanced design, mixed models were used initially, incorporating random (`group' and `nest' where appropriate) as well as xed e ects. Variance components were estimated using the residual maximum likelihood (REML) procedure in Genstat, and xed e ects from weighted least squares. However, models were simpli ed by omitting random e ects if there was no evidence of dependency. This was assessed by examining the change in deviance, which approximates to a chi-square distribution, when the random e ect was dropped from a full model. Speci- c details on the analysis of nest success, edgling weight and edgling survival are given separately below. Hatching and edging success There was no evidence of intragroup dependence between years, as dropping the random e ect `group' from the initial linear mixed model resulted in a non-signi cant change in deviance. Consequently, because the responses for hatching and edging success were discrete with a limited number of values (0, 1, 2, 3), they were treated as ordinal categorical responses in a proportional odds regression (McCullagh & Nelder 1989; Genstat Committee 1993). These models are extensions of generalized linear models using a logit link function, and describe the cumulative probability of the response

4 717 S. Legge being in each ordinal category given the signi cant covariates. The statistical signi cance of terms was assessed using the change in deviance. Fledgling weight Kookaburras exhibit reverse sexual dimorphism, which is evident even at edging (65%; S. Legge, unpublished data). Each edgling's weight was divided by the average weight for its sex and this corrected `weight ratio' was used as the response variable in a linear mixed model, with `group' and `nest' speci ed as the random e ects. To assess the signi cance of xed e ects, the deviance explained by a full model was contrasted with the deviance of a submodel with the same variance components but excluding the variable of interest. Fledgling survival A generalized linear mixed model with a binomial distribution was used to model the probability that a edgling would survive to independence. The variance components for `group' and `nest' were again estimated with the REML procedure; the signi cance of xed e ects was assessed using the Wald statistic when the e ect of interest was last in the model (Genstat Committee 1993). VARIABLES OF INTEREST The following variables were considered in all models: year, hatch date, family type, pair duration, the age of the breeding female, group size, and hatching sex ratio (% male). Groups were de ned as full families if all helpers were full o spring of the breeding pair, and step-families if one (or more) helper(s) was unrelated to one of the breeders; 14/ 126 group-years involved step-families. Pair duration had two levels, `novice' or `experienced', depending whether the pair had prior breeding experience together. Because the birds in a breeding pair were usually of similar age, only the age of the female was used in analysis. Group size was considered as a dichotomous factor (pair or group) and as a multilevel categorical variable because the e ect of additional helpers may be not be additive in a simple way. The models for edgling weight and edgling survival also considered the edging success of the nest, the edging sex ratio, and the sex of the individual edgling. Finally, the weight ratio of each edgling was considered in the model for edgling survival. Interactions were tested where pertinent. For example, helpers may only have a positive e ect in years when conditions are poor, and the weight ratios of male and female nestlings may be a ected di erently by variation in group size. PAIRED COMPARISONS The modelling analysis indicated that helpers had a positive e ect on edging success and edgling weight. To check whether this resulted from the confounding e ects of pair and/or territory quality, a restricted subset of the data was used for withingroup paired comparisons (Wilcoxon's paired-sample by rank test). Fledging success and the mean edgling weight ratio for the brood were compared for the same group (i.e. same breeding pair on the same territory) when the group size had increased by the addition of one, two or three helpers. Each group was only used in one comparison. To separate the e ects of male and female helpers, the success of groups was compared over two breeding attempts when the number of helpers of one sex had increased, the number of the other sex having remained constant. Again, this included increases of one, two or three males (or females). Comparisons went forwards and backwards in time, thus removing the potentially confounding e ects of year (e.g. one group might be compared in 1996 vs. 1995, another group in 1994 vs. 1997, etc.). Results Kookaburras in the study area occupied large territories (mean 69 ha, range 16±224 ha, 30 territories from 1997). Territory size was positively correlated with group size (F 1,29 ˆ 204, P <0001, r 2 ˆ 08; Fig. 1). Hatching began between the 11 and 16 October each year, and 75% of clutches had hatched within 37, 31 and 17 days of the rst clutch hatching in 1995, 1996 and 1997, respectively. The modal clutch size was three eggs (752% of 89 clutches); 165% of the 254 eggs did not hatch, mainly because of infertility or developmental failure (23 eggs in 20 clutches). Other causes of egg loss included egg breakage (eight in four clutches) and egg tossing by unknown individuals ( ve in three clutches were found on the ground under the nest). Loss of eggs to predators was rare: six eggs (23%) in two clutches from the same group suddenly disappeared, implying predation; there were three birds in this group. Just over half of all eggs resulted in a edgling (Table 1). The largest loss in potential productivity occurred during the nestling phase: although 165% (42/254) of eggs failed to hatch, 321% (68/212) of hatchlings failed to edge. Loss of young also occurred between the time young edged until nutritional independence at 2 months: 20% (24/125) of edglings died in this period. Nestling losses were attributed to predation (healthy nestling suddenly disappeared, 3/68, 44%), disease (nestling sickly on previous visit, 6/68, 88%), accident (nest caved in, branch broke, 7/68,

5 718 Reproductive success in kookaburras Fig. 1. Territory size (in hectares) against group size measured in September %), abandonment (brood found dead in nest, 15/68, 221%) or brood reduction (nestling underweight or injured from sibling aggression on previous visit, 37/68, 544%). In a subset of 72 nests where at least two eggs hatched, intensive observations revealed that 30 nests (417%) experienced brood reduction due either to sibling aggression (21/ 72 nests, 292%) or starvation (12/63 nests, 19%; S. Legge 2000b). Young nestlings were extremely aggressive to each other, often continuing to ght after being brought down to the ground temporarily to be weighed. A few nests experienced both early siblicide and late starvation. Fig. 2. Summary of the relationship between group size and (a) clutch size, (b) hatching success and (c) edging success. Bars represent means and SE of data, sample sizes are given above each bar. HATCHING AND FLEDGING SUCCESS Proportional odds regression showed that hatching success was signi cantly a ected by group size (89 nests; w 52 ˆ 121, P <005; Fig. 2b). Groups of two to six birds were most likely to hatch three nestlings and very unlikely to hatch none. However, there was a sharp reduction in hatching success for groups of seven or more, which were much more likely to experience complete hatching failure, and very unlikely to hatch three nestlings (Fig. 3a). This was not explained by di erences in clutch size, which varied little with group size (Fig. 2a). The sample of groups of seven was small (four groups), but hatch failure in each case was caused by damage to eggs. Of 10 eggs hatched to groups of seven or more, six were dented or broken. In contrast, in groups of six or less, only two eggs out of 244 failed to hatch because of damage (contingency w 12 ˆ 344, P < 0001). Year, hatch date, family type, pair duration, Table 1. Distribution of edging and hatching success for 89 clutches, for which the clutch size, hatching success and edging success were all known Distribution of clutch, nestling and edging sizes Total eggs, nestlings, edglings % surviving from eggs Clutch size Number at hatching Number at edging % surviving from nestlings

6 719 S. Legge Fig. 3. Model predictions for (a) the probability of hatching 0, 1, 2 or 3 nestlings, and (b) the probability of edging 0, 1, 2 or 3 young. Hatching and edging probabilities are given for groups of di erent size. age of the dominant female, the hatching sex ratio and interactions were all non-signi cant. Fledging success was also signi cantly a ected by group size (131 nests; w 52 ˆ 117, P <005; Fig. 2c). As group size increased from two to six birds, groups were less likely to experience complete edging failure, and more likely to edge three young (Fig. 3b). Once again, the largest groups of seven or more birds were least likely to edge young, re ecting their poor hatching success. Year, hatch date, family type, pair duration, age of the dominant female, the hatching sex ratio and interactions were non-signi cant. FLEDGLING WEIGHT The weight ratio of a edgling decreased with the number of siblings that edged together (155 edglings from 76 nests; w 22 ˆ 85, P <0025), indicating a trade-o between numbers of young and their mass (Fig. 4a). The weight ratio of edglings from unassisted pairs was lower than for groups of three or more birds (w 12 ˆ 171, P <0001). There was a non-signi cant trend for this e ect to be more pronounced for female edglings (interaction between edgling sex and group type: w 12 ˆ 27, P <01; Fig. 4b). Year, hatch date, family type, pair duration, age of the female breeder, hatching and edging sex ratios and interactions were non-signi cant. FLEDGLING SURVIVAL Heavier edglings had a higher chance of surviving to independence at 2 months (111 edglings; logistic regression w 12 ˆ 81, P<0005). The model predicted that 83% of edglings of average weight for their

7 720 Reproductive success in kookaburras Fig. 4. Fledgling weight ratio (weight of edgling divided by the mean weight for its sex) (a) against number of edglings in the brood and (b) of males and females in unassisted pairs vs. groups. sex (i.e. weight ratio ˆ 1) would survive to independence. A 10% reduction in weight ratio reduced survival probability to 72%, and a 20% weight ratio reduction reduced survival probability to 58%. In contrast, a 20% increase in weight ratio improved the probability of survival by 11%. The data suggested that females were more prone to mortality than males: of 61 male edglings with reliable survival data, eight died (131%). In contrast, 16/64 female edglings died (25%). (Note this sample is slightly larger than that used in the model of weight ratio because it includes edglings for which no weight ratio was available). However, neither edgling sex nor its interaction with weight ratio were signi cant in the model. Year, hatch date, family type, pair experience, age of the female breeder, hatching and edging sex ratios and interactions were all non-signi cant. PAIRED ANALYSIS ± DO HELPERS REALLY HELP? Paired comparisons showed that within groups, group size did not a ect edging success (Fig. 5a; Wilcoxon's paired sample by rank test T ˆ 255, P <018, n ˆ 24 pairs; 13/24 forward in time) nor the mean weight ratio of edglings (T ˆ 5, P <082, n ˆ 16 pairs; 9/16 forward in time). In comparisons Fig. 5. Di erences in the number of young edged when (a) group size increases by one to three helpers, (b) male helpers increase by one to three (number of female helpers unchanged), and (c) female helpers increase by one to three (number of male helpers unchanged). Symmetrical distributions around the dotted line indicate no consistent change in the number of young edged. within groups where the number of female helpers remained constant but male helpers varied, an increase in male helpers also had no e ect on edging success (Fig. 5b; T ˆ 15, P <089, n ˆ 16 pairs; 8/16 forward in time) nor mean weight ratio of edglings (T ˆ 115, P <02, n ˆ 9 pairs; 5/9 forward in time). In contrast, if the number of male helpers in a group remained constant but the number of female helpers increased, edging success signi cantly decreased (Fig. 5c; T ˆ 105, P <003, n ˆ 7 pairs; 3/7 forward in time). Although the available sample for this last test was small, edging success decreased for ve of the seven groups when the number of females increased, and the remaining two groups showed no change. Note that groups of seven birds, which showed markedly reduced reproductive success, were not included in any of the paired comparisons. The negative e ect of female helpers appeared to operate during the nestling

8 721 S. Legge phase rather than at the egg stage, as an increase in female helpers did not have any e ect on the hatching success of a group (T ˆ 3, P <05, n ˆ 6 pairs; 3/6 forward in time). Data were insu cient to test whether female helpers also had a detrimental e ect on edgling weight ratios. Discussion The data presented here reveal four interesting results. First, the paired analysis showed that between the usual group sizes of two to six birds, additional helpers did not improve the success of nesting attempts. Secondly, helpers had di erent e ects on nest success depending on their sex. Male helpers had a neutral e ect, whereas female helpers actually reduced edging success. Thirdly, unusually large kookaburra groups su ered drastically reduced nest success. Finally, the quality of the breeding pair and/or their territory clearly had a strong e ect on the reproductive success of their group, generating a non-causal correlation between group size and edging success, edging weight and, by extension, edgling survival. These four points are discussed in turn. GROUP SIZE DOES NOT AFFECT NEST SUCCESS The productivity of kookaburra nests is strongly in uenced by food availability. Nearly half of all nests experienced brood reduction, losing at least one nestling. Sometimes nestlings were lost sequentially, youngest to eldest, until the entire attempt failed. Young that edged below average weight were more likely to die before reaching independence, and edgling weight decreased with the number of siblings, indicating that kookaburras have trouble raising a full brood. Also, the extra cost of raising a female (the larger sex) is su ered disproportionately by `poorer' groups (pairs). Besides alleviating brood reduction, kookaburra helpers lack other avenues to increase nest success. Helpers in some co-operatively breeding birds reduce predation of eggs and nestlings (Austad & Rabenold 1985; Woolfenden & Fitzpatrick 1990), but like many hole-nesters (Emlen & Wrege 1991) nest-predation is rare in kookaburras. The prevalence of brood reduction in kookaburras suggested that extra help feeding young would translate into higher success. Yet additional helpers were not associated with greater nest success. This result concurs with an analysis of feeding contributions presented elsewhere, showing that group size did not a ect the overall amount of food delivered to the nest. In larger groups, breeders reduced their own contribution to compensate completely for the e orts of their helpers (Legge 2000a). Why do kookaburras miss this opportunity to boost productivity? There are two possibilities. Helpers may be ine ectual at preventing early siblicide, which accounts for three-quarters of all the nestlings lost through brood reduction. Early siblicide occurs soon after hatching, well before food demands peak, and is heavily in uenced by, for example, the degree of hatch asynchrony and nestling sex (Legge 2000b). Thus competitive interactions between nestlings may be more important than group size in determining nestling loss (Innes & Johnston 1996). However, hatching asynchrony and other factors a ecting the outcome of sibling rivalry, including group size, are probably modi ed according to the group's ability to rear its young (Legge 2000b). In addition, some nestlings die from starvation, rather than sibling aggression, later in the nestling period. Alternatively, if provisioning young is costly, then the bene ts of reducing workloads as group size increases may out-weigh the bene ts of edging more young for both breeders and helpers (Boland et al. 1997; Hatchwell 1999; Heinsohn & Legge 1999). The corollary to this is that breeders with helpers should enjoy greater survivorship. Kookaburras are long-lived birds (15 years; Higgins 1999) and data on survival from this 4-year study are weak: only 10 breeders disappeared during the entire period, and only four of these were con rmed as dead (from recovered carcasses). Moreover, three out of these four birds died from collisions with cars. FEMALE HELPERS REDUCE NEST SUCCESS Female helpers may reduce nest success because they disrupt breeding attempts. Other group members, especially the breeding female, may perceive female helpers as reproductive competition and spend time chasing them away (Gaston 1978b; Stallcup & Woolfenden 1978; Mumme, Koenig & Pitelka 1983; McGowan & Woolfenden 1990). However, antagonistic behaviour around nests was never observed. As well as their di ering e ects on nest success, male and female helpers also di er in their helping e ort: males incubate eggs and provision nestlings at a higher rate than females (Legge 2000a). If other group members use group size to gauge their appropriate level of feeding, they may overvalue female contributions to the detriment of the corporate e ort. TOO MANY HELPERS SPOIL NEST SUCCESS Although the available sample was small, the largest group sizes of seven birds experienced drastically reduced nest success because eggs were much more likely to be damaged (Figs 2b, 3a). Large groups tend to have more female helpers than small groups, but it seems unlikely that egg damage was caused by female competition (Mumme et al. 1983). In within-

9 722 Reproductive success in kookaburras group paired comparisons, an increase in the number of female helpers did not reduce hatching success, although it did reduce edging success. Also, DNA ngerprinting showed that female helpers never gain maternity in nests (Legge & Cockburn 2000). It seems more likely that breakages occur more frequently when incubation is co-ordinated between many birds (Koenig 1982; Komdeur 1994). intruders (McComb, Packer & Pusey 1994). Parry (1973) noted that changes in group size were associated with concomitant changes in territory size (see also Gaston 1978c; Brooker & Rowley 1995). During this study, territory boundaries did not appear to shift conspicuously, but territory expansions may require neighbouring territories to retract or `die'. PAIR AND TERRITORY QUALITY Group size had no e ect on either edging success or edgling weight in paired comparisons that controlled for the quality of the breeding pair and their territory. Yet the results of the mixed model indicated that group size was associated with higher edging success and edgling weight. This highlights the problems inherent in correlational studies of the e ect of helpers on reproductive success, and demonstrates that extreme caution is required when interpreting the results of such studies. If territory quality is important in kookaburras, it seems strange that other ecological variables tested in the mixed model were non-signi cant. There were no interannual di erences in edging success or edgling weight despite the fact that the study spanned years of extremely low and high rainfall. Similarly, hatch date was not signi cant, although seasonal e ects are known to be strong in other species. However, most nests hatched within a very short period (e.g. in 1997, 50% of all clutches hatched within 7 days), possibly dampening variability in conditions experienced between nests with di erent hatch dates. In addition, the kookaburra's diet is extremely broad (reviewed in Higgins 1999), and breeding birds may be able to switch prey types in response to short-term uctuations in availability. The earliest hatch date occurred between 11 and 16 October in every year of the study, despite the variation in environmental conditions between years, again suggesting a compensatory response to any short-falls. The consistently high nest success of some breeding pairs may be explained by the size of their territory, as larger groups live on larger territories (Parry 1973; Reyer & Schmidl 1988; this study). Although kookaburras hunt a wide variety of prey, presumably the absolute biomass is greater on larger territories. Helpers may be important for maintaining ownership of territories, because they contribute conspicuously to territory defence. Intruders are aggressively repelled, the borders with neighbours are patrolled during ritualized displays, and groups regularly chorus from roost sites at dawn and dusk (Parry 1973; Reyer & Schmidl 1988; S. Legge, unpublished data). The duration and loudness of choruses is positively related to group size (Reyer & Schmidl 1988), and chorusing probably conveys information about numbers to neighbours and OTHER REASONS TO HELP Helping kin may occur for reasons other than gaining indirect tness bene ts. Helping may be an unselected response to stimuli normally presented during parenthood (Jamieson 1991). This seems unlikely in kookaburras, because the feeding contribution of helpers is plastic, modi ed according to their group size, breeding status and sex (Legge 2000a). Alternatively, helping may provide valuable breeding experience (Komdeur 1996). However, kookaburras breeding together for the rst time did not fare worse than experienced pairs, nor did the age of breeders a ect any aspect of reproduction, suggesting breeding success does not improve with experience. The measure of experience (pair duration) used in the analysis may be too crude, as `novice' pairs may nevertheless have prior breeding experience with other partners, or prior helping experience; unfortunately only one pair was known of during the study that formed from completely inexperienced birds. Conclusions This study aimed to nd whether kookaburras gained indirect tness bene ts from helping to raise siblings, as other bene ts of helping such as territorial inheritance or mating opportunities are not available to them (Legge & Cockburn 2000). Helpers were unable to increase the number of nesting attempts in a season, because kookaburras are single-brooded. Surprisingly, despite the high frequency of brood reduction, helpers did not increase the number or weight of young in each nesting attempt. Indeed, very large groups su ered reduced hatching success and female helpers exacted a toll on edging success. Although it was not possible to test whether helper contributions increased the survivorship of breeders, the alacrity of all kookaburras to reduce their workload rather than alleviate brood reduction suggests that raising young is costly. It therefore seems likely that breeders gain an energetic advantage from having helpers. This study is unusual because the e ect of helpers on nest success was examined after controlling for pair and territory quality. As well as revealing the absence of a group size e ect, when this controlled analysis was compared to a more extensive correlational analysis, pair and territory quality were shown to have a con-

10 723 S. Legge siderable impact on nest success. The importance of such quality e ects for the reproductive success of co-operative breeders is often discussed, but rarely demonstrated. Acknowledgements I thank A. Cockburn, E. Doerr, V. Doerr, two anonymous referees and especially R. Heinsohn for comments on the manuscript, and R. Cunningham and C. Donnelly for statistical advice. Canberra Nature Park, the Australian National Botanic Gardens and the Australian Bird and Bat Banding Scheme (ABBBS) were always helpful. I was supported by Overseas Postgraduate Research and ANU Scholarships. The work was carried out under permit from the ANU Animal Experimentation Ethics Committee, ABBBS and ACT Parks and Conservation. References Austad, S.N. & Rabenold, K.N. (1985) Reproductive enhancement by helpers and an experimental inquiry into its mechanism in the bicolored wren. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 17, 19±27. Boland, C.R.J., Heinsohn, R. & Cockburn, A. (1997) Experimental manipulation of brood reduction and parental care in co-operatively breeding white-winged choughs. Ecology, 66, 683±691. Brooker, M. & Rowley, I. (1995) The signi cance of territory size and quality in the mating strategy of the splendid fairy-wren. Ecology, 64, 614±627. Brown, J.L. (1987) Helping and Communal Breeding in Birds: Ecology and Evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Brown, J.L. & Brown, E.R. (1981) Kin selection and individual selection in babblers. Natural Selection and Social Behaviour (eds R.D. Alexander & D.W. Tinkle). Chiron Press, New York, NY. Brown, J.L., Brown, E.R., Brown, S.D. & Dow, D.D. (1982) Helpers: e ects of experimental removal on reproductive success. Science, 215, 421±422. Cockburn, A. (1998) Evolution of helping behaviour in cooperatively breeding birds. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 29, 141±177. Craig, J.L. (1984) Are communal pukeko caught in the prisoner's dilemma? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 14, 147±150. Crick, H.Q.P. (1992) Load-lightening in cooperatively breeding birds and the cost of reproduction. Ibis, 134, 56±61. Ekman, J., Bylin, A. & TegelstroÈ m, H. (1999) Increased lifetime reproductive success for Siberian jay (Perisoreus infaustus) males with delayed dispersal. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, Biological Sciences, 266, 911±915. Emlen, S.T. (1997) Predicting family dynamics in social vertebrates. Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach, 4th edn (eds J.R. Krebs & N.B. Davies), pp. 228±253. Blackwell Science, Oxford, UK. Emlen, S.T. & Wrege, P. (1991) Breeding biology of whitefronted bee-eaters at Nakuru: the in uence of helpers on breeder tness. Ecology, 60, 309± 326. Gaston, A.J. (1978a) Demography of the jungle babbler Turdoides striatus. Ecology, 47, 845± 870. Gaston, A.J. (1978b) Ecology of the common babbler Turdoides caudatus. Ibis, 120, 415±432. Gaston, A.J. (1978c) The evolution of group territorial behaviour and cooperative breeding. American Naturalist, 112, 1091±1100. Genstat Committee (1993) Genstat 5 Release 3 Reference Manual. Clarendon Press, Oxford, UK. Green, D.J., Cockburn, A., Hall, M.L., Osmond, H.L. & Dunn, P.O. (1995) Increased opportunities for cuckoldry may be why dominant male fairy-wrens tolerate helpers. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, Biological Sciences, 262, 297±303. Gri ths, R., Double, M.C., Orr, K. & Dawson, R.J.G. (1998) A simple DNA test to sex most birds. Molecular Ecology, 7, 1071±1075. Hamilton, W.D. (1964) The genetical evolution of social behaviour. II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 17±52. Hatchwell, B.J. (1999) Investment strategies of breeders in avian cooperative breeding systems. American Naturalist, 154, 205±219. Heinsohn, R.G. (1991) Slow learning of foraging skills and extended parental care in cooperatively breeding whitewinged choughs. American Naturalist, 137, 864±881. Heinsohn, R.G. (1992) Cooperative enhancement of reproductive success in white-winged choughs. Evolutionary Ecology, 6, 97±114. Heinsohn, R. & Legge, S. (1999) The cost of helping. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 14, 53±57. Higgins, P.J. (1999) Laughing Kookaburra. Handbook of Australian, New Zealand, and Antarctic Birds. Oxford University Press, Melbourne, Australia. Innes, K.E. & Johnston, R.E. (1996) Cooperative breeding in the white-throated magpie-jay. How do auxiliaries in uence nesting success? Animal Behaviour, 51, 519± 533. Jamieson, I.G. (1991) The unselected hypothesis for the evolution of helping behavior: too much or too little emphasis on natural selection? American Naturalist, 138, 271±282. Koenig, W.D. (1982) Ecological and social factors a ecting hatchability of eggs. Auk, 99, 526±536. Komdeur, J. (1994) Experimental evidence for helping and hindering by previous o spring in the cooperativebreeding Seychelles warbler Acrocephalus sechellensis. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 34, 175±186. Komdeur, J. (1996) In uence of helping and breeding experience on reproductive performance in the Seychelles warbler: a translocation experiment. Behavioral Ecology, 7, 326±333. Legge, S. (2000a) Helper contributions in the laughing kookaburra: feeding young is no laughing matter. Animal Behaviour, in press. Legge, S. (2000b) Siblicide in the cooperatively breeding laughing kookaburra. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology, in press. Legge, S. & Cockburn, A. (2000) The social and mating system of the cooperatively breeding laughing kookaburra. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 47, 220± 229. Leonard, M.L., Horn, A.G. & Eden, S.F. (1989) Does juvenile helping enhance breeder reproductive success? A removal experiment on moorhens. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 25, 357±362. Lewis, D.M. (1981) Determinants of reproductive success of the white-browed sparrow weaver, Plocepasser mahali. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 9, 83±93.

11 724 Reproductive success in kookaburras Ligon, J.D. & Ligon, S.H. (1990) Green woodhoopoes: life history traits and sociality. Cooperative Breeding in Birds (eds P.B. Stacey & W.D. Koenig), pp. 33±65. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK. McComb, K., Packer, C. & Pusey, A. (1994) Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leo. Animal Behaviour, 47, 379± 387. McCullagh, P. & Nelder, J.A. (1989) Generalized Linear Models. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK. McGowan, K.J. & Woolfenden, G.E. (1990) Contributions to edgling feeding in the Florida scrub jay. Journal of Animal Ecology, 59, 691±707. Magrath, D. & Yezerinac, S.M. (1997) Facultative helping does not in uence reproductive success or survival in cooperatively breeding white-browed scrubwrens. Journal of Animal Ecology, 66, 658±670. Monadjem, A., Owen-Smith, N. & Kemp, A.C. (1995) Aspects of the breeding biology of the arrowmarked babbler Turdoides jardineii in South Africa. Ibis, 137, 515±518. Mumme, R.L. (1992) Do helpers increase reproductive success ± an experimental analysis in the Florida scrub jay. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 31, 319±328. Mumme, R.L., Koenig, W.D. & Pitelka, F.A. (1983) Reproductive competition in the acorn woodpecker: sisters destroy each other's eggs. Nature, 306, 583±584. Mumme, R.L., Koenig, W.D. & Ratnieks, F.L.W. (1989) Helping behaviour, reproductive value, and the future component of direct tness. Animal Behaviour, 38, 331±343. Parry, V. (1973) The auxiliary social system and its e ect on territory and breeding in kookaburras. Emu, 73, 81±100. Reyer, H.-U., Dittami, J.P. & Hall, M.R. (1986) Avian helpers at the nest: are they psychologically castrated? Ethology, 71, 216±228. Reyer, H.-U. & Schmidl, D. (1988) Helpers have little to laugh about: group structure and vocalisation in the laughing kookaburra Dacelo novaeguineae. Emu, 88, 150±160. Sherley, G.H. (1989) Bene ts of courtship-feeding for ri eman (Acanthisitta chloris) parents. Behaviour, 109, 309±318. Stallcup, J.A. & Woolfenden, G.E. (1978) Family status and contributions to breeding by Florida scrub jays. Animal Behaviour, 26, 1144±1156. Sydeman, W.J., Guntert, M. & Balda, R.P. (1988) Annual reproductive yield in the cooperative pygmy nuthatch (Sitta pygmaea). Auk, 105, 70±77. Woolfenden, G.E. & Fitzpatrick, J.W. (1990) Florida scrub-jays: a synopsis after 18 years study. Cooperative Breeding in Birds: Long-Term Studies of Ecology and Behaviour (eds P.B. Stacey & W.D. Koenig), pp. 239± 266. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK. Received 13 September 1999; revision received 7 February 2000

KAZUHIRO EGUCHI*, SATOSHI YAMAGISHI, SHIGEKI ASAI, HISASHI NAGATA and TERUAKI HINO

KAZUHIRO EGUCHI*, SATOSHI YAMAGISHI, SHIGEKI ASAI, HISASHI NAGATA and TERUAKI HINO Ecology 2002 71, Helping does not enhance reproductive success of Blackwell Science Ltd cooperatively breeding rufous vanga in Madagascar KAZUHIRO EGUCHI*, SATOSHI YAMAGISHI, SHIGEKI ASAI, HISASHI NAGATA

More information

Effects of helpers on breeder survival in the red-cockaded woodpecker (Picoides borealis)

Effects of helpers on breeder survival in the red-cockaded woodpecker (Picoides borealis) Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2002) 51:336 344 DOI 10.1007/s00265-001-0441-3 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Memuna Z. Khan Jeffrey R. Walters Effects of helpers on breeder survival in the red-cockaded woodpecker (Picoides borealis)

More information

Incubation feeding by helpers influences female nest attendance in the green woodhoopoe, Phoeniculus purpureus

Incubation feeding by helpers influences female nest attendance in the green woodhoopoe, Phoeniculus purpureus Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2004) 55:583 588 DOI 10.1007/s00265-003-0747-4 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Andrew N. Radford Incubation feeding by helpers influences female nest attendance in the green woodhoopoe, Phoeniculus

More information

REPRODUCTION OF THE RED-COCKADED WOODPECKER IN CENTRAL FLORIDA

REPRODUCTION OF THE RED-COCKADED WOODPECKER IN CENTRAL FLORIDA Wilson Bull., 104(2), 1992, pp. 285-294 REPRODUCTION OF THE RED-COCKADED WOODPECKER IN CENTRAL FLORIDA ROY S. DELOTELLE AND ROBERT J. EFTING~ ABSTRACT. - Red-cockaded Woodpeckers (Picoides borealis) near

More information

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153) i Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN 978-1-927194-58-4, page 153) Activity 9: Intraspecific relationships extra questions

More information

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns Demography and Populations Survivorship Demography is the study of fecundity and survival Four critical variables Age of first breeding Number of young fledged each year Juvenile survival Adult survival

More information

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition Proceedings of The National Conference on Undergraduate Research (NCUR) 2003 University of Utah, Salt Lake City, Utah March 13-15, 2003 Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris):

More information

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories Chapters 12 16 Many details in book, esp know: Chpt 12 pg 338-345, 359-365 Chpt 13 pg 367-373, 377-381, 385-391 Table 13-1 Chpt 14 pg 420-422, 427-430 Chpt 15 pg 431-438,

More information

Co-operative breeding by Long-tailed Tits

Co-operative breeding by Long-tailed Tits Co-operative breeding by Long-tailed Tits v N. W. Glen and C. M. Perrins For most of this century, ornithologists have tended to believe that the majority of birds breed monogamously, with either the pair

More information

Brooding, provisioning, and compensatory care in the cooperatively breeding acorn woodpecker

Brooding, provisioning, and compensatory care in the cooperatively breeding acorn woodpecker Behavioral Ecology doi:10.1093/beheco/arr172 Advance Access publication 24 October 2011 Original Article Brooding, provisioning, and compensatory care in the cooperatively breeding acorn woodpecker Walter

More information

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories Chapters 12 17 Read the book many details Courtship and Mating Breeding systems Sex Nests and Incubation Parents and their Offspring Overview Passion Field trips and the

More information

DO DIFFERENT CLUTCH SIZES OF THE TREE SWALLOW (Tachycineta bicolor)

DO DIFFERENT CLUTCH SIZES OF THE TREE SWALLOW (Tachycineta bicolor) DO DIFFERENT CLUTCH SIZES OF THE TREE SWALLOW (Tachycineta bicolor) HAVE VARYING FLEDGLING SUCCESS? Cassandra Walker August 25 th, 2017 Abstract Tachycineta bicolor (Tree Swallow) were surveyed over a

More information

University of Groningen. Offspring fitness and individual optimization of clutch size Both, C; Tinbergen, Joost; Noordwijk, Arie J.

University of Groningen. Offspring fitness and individual optimization of clutch size Both, C; Tinbergen, Joost; Noordwijk, Arie J. University of Groningen Offspring fitness and individual optimization of clutch size Both, C; Tinbergen, Joost; Noordwijk, Arie J. van Published in: Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B,

More information

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production May 2013 Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production Michael Longley, Global Technical Transfer Manager Summary Introduction Chick numbers are most often reduced during the period

More information

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production Michael Longley, Global Technical Transfer Manager May 2013 SUMMARY Introduction Chick numbers are most often reduced during the period

More information

An Experimental Study of Chick Provisioning in the Cooperatively Breeding Acorn Woodpecker

An Experimental Study of Chick Provisioning in the Cooperatively Breeding Acorn Woodpecker Ethology An Experimental Study of Chick Provisioning in the Cooperatively Breeding Acorn Woodpecker Walter D. Koenig* & Eric L. Walters * Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA Department of Neurobiology

More information

Factors Influencing Local Recruitment in Tree Swallows, Tachycineta bicolor

Factors Influencing Local Recruitment in Tree Swallows, Tachycineta bicolor Grand Valley State University ScholarWorks@GVSU Honors Projects Undergraduate Research and Creative Practice 2013 Factors Influencing Local Recruitment in Tree Swallows, Tachycineta bicolor Danielle M.

More information

Warmer springs lead to mistimed reproduction in great tits (Parus major) Visser, M.E.; Noordwijk, A.J. van; Tinbergen, Joost; Lessells, C.M.

Warmer springs lead to mistimed reproduction in great tits (Parus major) Visser, M.E.; Noordwijk, A.J. van; Tinbergen, Joost; Lessells, C.M. University of Groningen Warmer springs lead to mistimed reproduction in great tits (Parus major) Visser, M.E.; Noordwijk, A.J. van; Tinbergen, Joost; Lessells, C.M. Published in: Proceedings of the Royal

More information

Influence of kinship on helping behavior in Galfipagos mockingbirds

Influence of kinship on helping behavior in Galfipagos mockingbirds Behav Ecol Sociobiol (1988) 22:14~ 152 Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 9 Springer-Verlag 1988 nfluence of kinship on helping behavior in Galfipagos mockingbirds Robert L. Curry* Department of Biology,

More information

Cooperative Breeding by the Galfipagos Mockingbird, Nesomimus parvulus

Cooperative Breeding by the Galfipagos Mockingbird, Nesomimus parvulus Behav Ecol Sociobiol (1982) 10:65 73 Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 9 Springer-Verlag 1982 Cooperative Breeding by the Galfipagos Mockingbird, Nesomimus parvulus Margaret F. Kinnaird and Peter R.

More information

Australian Journal of Zoology

Australian Journal of Zoology CSIRO PUBLISHING Australian Journal of Zoology Volume 47, 1999 CSIRO Australia 1999 A journal for the publication of the results of original scientific research in all branches of zoology, except the taxonomy

More information

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl) Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl) Family: Strigidae (Typical Owls) Order: Strigiformes (Owls) Class: Aves (Birds) Fig. 1. Mottled owl, Ciccaba virgata. [http://www.owling.com/mottled13.htm, downloaded 12 November

More information

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS Nov., 1965 505 BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS Lack ( 1954; 40-41) has pointed out that in species of birds which have asynchronous hatching, brood size may be adjusted

More information

Pair bond and breeding success in Blue Tits Parus caeruleus and Great Tits Parus major

Pair bond and breeding success in Blue Tits Parus caeruleus and Great Tits Parus major Ibis (25), 147, 92 18 Blackwell Publishing, Ltd. Pair bond and breeding success in s Parus caeruleus and s Parus major MIRIAM PAMPUS*, KARL-HEINZ SCHMIDT & WOLFGANG WILTSCHKO Fachbereich Biologie der J.W.

More information

Ontario Gray Jays Help on the World Stage: Part 2

Ontario Gray Jays Help on the World Stage: Part 2 15 Ontario Gray Jays Help on the World Stage: Part 2 Dan Strickland In Part 1 of this article (Ontario Birds 20: 130-138), I stated that a common Ontario bird, the Gray Jay (Perisoreus canadensis), provides

More information

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis I. P. JOHNSON and R. M. SIBLY Fourteen individually marked pairs o f Canada Geese were observedfrom January to April on their feeding grounds

More information

Corresponding author: Daniela Canestrari, address: Dep. of Zoology, University of

Corresponding author: Daniela Canestrari, address: Dep. of Zoology, University of False feedings at the nests of carrion crows Corvus corone corone 1 DANIELA CANESTRARI 1,2, JOSÉ M. MARCOS 3, VITTORIO BAGLIONE 4 1 Dep. of Zoology, University of Cambridge 2 Dep. of Animal Biology, University

More information

Correlates of group size in a cooperatively breeding cichlid fish (Neolamprologus pulcher)

Correlates of group size in a cooperatively breeding cichlid fish (Neolamprologus pulcher) Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2001) 50:134 140 DOI 10.1007/s002650100343 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Sigal Balshine Brenda Leach Francis Neat Hannah Reid Michael Taborsky Noam Werner Correlates of group size in a cooperatively

More information

Offspring sex ratio in red-winged blackbirds is dependent on

Offspring sex ratio in red-winged blackbirds is dependent on Proc. Nati. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 80, pp. 6141-6145, October 1983 Population Biology Offspring sex ratio in red-winged blackbirds is dependent on maternal age (parental age/reproduction/offspring sex/population

More information

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Laying May May 2 to 26. Incubation Early May to mid June Early May to mid June 30 to 34

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Laying May May 2 to 26. Incubation Early May to mid June Early May to mid June 30 to 34 Snowy Owl Bubo scandiacus 1. INTRODUCTION s have a circumpolar distribution, breeding in Fennoscandia, Arctic Russia, Alaska, northern Canada and northeast Greenland. They are highly nomadic and may migrate

More information

Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) are breeding earlier at Creamer s Field Migratory Waterfowl Refuge, Fairbanks, AK

Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) are breeding earlier at Creamer s Field Migratory Waterfowl Refuge, Fairbanks, AK Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) are breeding earlier at Creamer s Field Migratory Waterfowl Refuge, Fairbanks, AK Abstract: We examined the average annual lay, hatch, and fledge dates of tree swallows

More information

University of Canberra. This thesis is available in print format from the University of Canberra Library.

University of Canberra. This thesis is available in print format from the University of Canberra Library. University of Canberra This thesis is available in print format from the University of Canberra Library. If you are the author of this thesis and wish to have the whole thesis loaded here, please contact

More information

Breeding White Storks( Ciconia ciconia at Chessington World of Adventures Paul Wexler

Breeding White Storks( Ciconia ciconia at Chessington World of Adventures Paul Wexler Breeding White Storks(Ciconia ciconia) at Chessington World of Adventures Paul Wexler The White Stork belongs to the genus Ciconia of which there are seven other species incorporated predominantly throughout

More information

Male parental care and monogamy in snow buntings

Male parental care and monogamy in snow buntings Behav Ecol Sociobiol (1987) 20:377-382 Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 9 Springer-Verlag 1987 Male parental care and monogamy in snow buntings Bruce E. Lyon*, Robert D. Montgomerie, and Linda D. Hamilton*

More information

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp SHORT COMMUNICATIONS 693 Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp. 693-697 Conspecific aggression in a Wood Stork colony in Georgia.-The probability of interactions among conspecifics, including aggression, is

More information

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Coraciformes Family: Dacelonidae Scientific Name: Dacelo novaeguineae Common Name: Laughing kookaburra

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Coraciformes Family: Dacelonidae Scientific Name: Dacelo novaeguineae Common Name: Laughing kookaburra Order: Coraciformes Family: Dacelonidae Scientific Name: Dacelo novaeguineae Common Name: Laughing kookaburra AZA Management: Green Yellow Red None Photo (Male): Photo (Female): NATURAL HISTORY: Geographic

More information

Food-supplementation does not override the e ect of egg mass on tness-related traits of nestling house wrens

Food-supplementation does not override the e ect of egg mass on tness-related traits of nestling house wrens Ecology 2000, 69, Food-supplementation does not override the e ect of egg mass on tness-related traits of nestling house wrens JOHN D. STYRSKY*, ROBERT C. DOBBS and CHARLES F. THOMPSON Behavior, Ecology,

More information

Increased predation on pukeko eggs after the application of rabbit control measures

Increased predation on pukeko eggs after the application of rabbit control measures 89 SHORT COMMUNICATION Increased predation on pukeko eggs after the application of rabbit control measures John Haselmayer 1 and Ian G. Jamieson* Department of Zoology, University of Otago, PO Box 56,

More information

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories Chapters 12 16 Read the book many details Courtship and Mating Breeding systems Sex Nests and Incubation Parents and their Offspring Outline 1. Pair formation or other

More information

Red Crowned Parakeet (Cyanoramphus novaezelandiae) health, disease and nesting study on Tiritiri Matangi 2014/2015. Emma Wells on behalf of

Red Crowned Parakeet (Cyanoramphus novaezelandiae) health, disease and nesting study on Tiritiri Matangi 2014/2015. Emma Wells on behalf of Red Crowned Parakeet (Cyanoramphus novaezelandiae) health, disease and nesting study on Tiritiri Matangi 2014/2015 John Sibley Emma Wells on behalf of Auckland Zoo, Supporters of Tiritiri Matangi, Massey

More information

Crotophaga major (Greater Ani)

Crotophaga major (Greater Ani) Crotophaga major (Greater Ani) Family: Cuculidae (Cuckoos and Anis) Order: Cuculiformes (Cuckoos, Anis and Turacos) Class: Aves (Birds) Fig. 1. Greater ani, Crotophaga major. [http://www.birdforum.net/opus/greater_ani,

More information

Sex-biased initial eggs favours sons in the slightly size-dimorphic Scops owl (Otus scops)

Sex-biased initial eggs favours sons in the slightly size-dimorphic Scops owl (Otus scops) Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2002, 76, 1 7. With 3 figures Sex-biased initial eggs favours sons in the slightly size-dimorphic Scops owl (Otus scops) G. BLANCO 1 *, J. A. DÁVILA 1, J. A.

More information

BLUEBIRD NEST BOX REPORT

BLUEBIRD NEST BOX REPORT BLUEBIRD NEST BOX REPORT - 2014 By Leo Hollein, August 29, 2014 Tree Swallows Thrive Bluebirds Struggle Weather has a major impact on wildlife including birds. However, not all nesting birds in the Refuge

More information

PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE

PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE Condor, 81:78-82 0 The Cooper Ornithological Society 1979 PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE SUSAN J. HANNON AND FRED C. ZWICKEL Parallel studies on increasing (Zwickel 1972) and decreasing

More information

Kori Bustard Husbandry. Sara Hallager, Biologist, Smithsonian National Zoological Park

Kori Bustard Husbandry. Sara Hallager, Biologist, Smithsonian National Zoological Park Kori Bustard Husbandry Sara Hallager, Biologist, Smithsonian National Zoological Park Ardeotis kori 2 subspecies [?] Africa s largest flying bird Captive males: 12-19kg Seasonal weight gain up to 4kg Captive

More information

AGE AT FIRST BREEDING AND CHANGE IN PLUMAGE OF KELP GULLS LARUS DOMINICANUS IN SOUTH AFRICA. R. J. M. CRAWFORD*, B. M. DYER* and L.

AGE AT FIRST BREEDING AND CHANGE IN PLUMAGE OF KELP GULLS LARUS DOMINICANUS IN SOUTH AFRICA. R. J. M. CRAWFORD*, B. M. DYER* and L. S. Afr. J. mar. Sci. 22: 27 32 2000 27 AGE AT FIRST BREEDING AND CHANGE IN PLUMAGE OF KELP GULLS LARUS DOMINICANUS IN SOUTH AFRICA R. J. M. CRAWFORD*, B. M. DYER* and L. UPFOLD* In South Africa, kelp gulls

More information

Caterpillar abundance in the territory affects the breeding performance of great tit Parus major minor

Caterpillar abundance in the territory affects the breeding performance of great tit Parus major minor Oecologia (1998) 114:514±521 Ó Springer-Verlag 1998 Shin-Ichi Seki á Hajime Takano Caterpillar abundance in the territory affects the breeding performance of great tit Parus major minor Received: 10 June

More information

Growth and Development. Embryonic development 2/22/2018. Timing of hatching. Hatching. Young birds and their parents

Growth and Development. Embryonic development 2/22/2018. Timing of hatching. Hatching. Young birds and their parents Growth and Development Young birds and their parents Embryonic development From fertilization to hatching, the embryo undergoes sequence of 42 distinct developmental stages The first 33 stages vary little

More information

The effects of environmental and individual quality on reproductive performance Amininasab, Seyed Mehdi

The effects of environmental and individual quality on reproductive performance Amininasab, Seyed Mehdi University of Groningen The effects of environmental and individual quality on reproductive performance Amininasab, Seyed Mehdi IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's

More information

Hole-nesting birds. In natural conditions great and blue tits breed in holes that are made by e.g. woodpeckers

Hole-nesting birds. In natural conditions great and blue tits breed in holes that are made by e.g. woodpeckers Hole-nesting birds In natural conditions great and blue tits breed in holes that are made by e.g. woodpeckers Norhern willow tits excavate their own holes in rotten trees and do not accept old holes or

More information

BREEDING ECOLOGY OF THE LITTLE TERN, STERNA ALBIFRONS PALLAS, 1764 IN SINGAPORE

BREEDING ECOLOGY OF THE LITTLE TERN, STERNA ALBIFRONS PALLAS, 1764 IN SINGAPORE NATURE IN SINGAPORE 2008 1: 69 73 Date of Publication: 10 September 2008 National University of Singapore BREEDING ECOLOGY OF THE LITTLE TERN, STERNA ALBIFRONS PALLAS, 1764 IN SINGAPORE J. W. K. Cheah*

More information

Red-Tailed Hawk Buteo jamaicensis

Red-Tailed Hawk Buteo jamaicensis Red-Tailed Hawk Buteo jamaicensis This large, dark headed, broad-shouldered hawk is one of the most common and widespread hawks in North America. The Red-tailed hawk belongs to the genus (family) Buteo,

More information

Record of Predation by Sugar Glider on Breeding Eastern Rosellas 33Km NE of Melbourne in November 2016

Record of Predation by Sugar Glider on Breeding Eastern Rosellas 33Km NE of Melbourne in November 2016 Record of Predation by Sugar Glider on Breeding Eastern Rosellas 33Km NE of Melbourne in November 2016 By Frank Pierce [email - jmandfp@bigpond.com.au ] 18/01/2016 SUMMARY Eastern Rosellas nested in a

More information

Costs and bene ts of surplus offspring in the lesser kestrel (Falco naumanni )

Costs and bene ts of surplus offspring in the lesser kestrel (Falco naumanni ) Behav Ecol Sociobiol (1997) 41: 129 ± 137 Ó Springer-Verlag 1997 Jose M. Aparicio Costs and bene ts of surplus offspring in the lesser kestrel (Falco naumanni ) Received: 17 January 1997 / Accepted after

More information

ANALYSIS OF GROWTH OF THE RED-TAILED HAWK 1

ANALYSIS OF GROWTH OF THE RED-TAILED HAWK 1 OhioJ. Sci. DEVONIAN ICROPHYTOPLANKTON 13 Copyright 1983 Ohio Acad. Sci. OO3O-O95O/83/OOO1-OO13 $2.00/0 ANALYSIS O GROWTH O THE RED-TAILED HAWK 1 ARK A. SPRINGER 2 and DAVID R. OSBORNE, Department of Zoology,

More information

PROTECTING MANLY S PENGUINS

PROTECTING MANLY S PENGUINS PROTECTING MANLY S PENGUINS NATIONAL PARKS AND WILDLIFE SERVICES VOLUNTARY PENGUIN WARDEN PROGRAM CONFERENCE PRESENTATION WEDNESDAY JULY 23 RD 2008 BACKGROUND: LITTLE PENGUINS EUDYPTULA MINOR o Manly s

More information

BREEDING BIOLOGY OF AMERICAN CROWS

BREEDING BIOLOGY OF AMERICAN CROWS Wilson Bull., 102(4), 1990, pp. 6 15-622 BREEDING BIOLOGY OF AMERICAN CROWS JUNE A. C~BERLAIN-AUGER, PETER J. AUGER,~ AND ERIC G. STRAUSS~ ABSTRACT.-The breeding biology of cooperatively breeding American

More information

Skill acquisition and the timing of natal dispersal in the white-throated magpie-jay, Calocitta formosa

Skill acquisition and the timing of natal dispersal in the white-throated magpie-jay, Calocitta formosa Anim. Behav., 1996, 51, 575 588 Skill acquisition and the timing of natal dispersal in the white-throated magpie-jay, Calocitta formosa TOM A. LANGEN Department of Biology, University of California at

More information

Below, we present the methods used to address these objectives, our preliminary results and next steps in this multi-year project.

Below, we present the methods used to address these objectives, our preliminary results and next steps in this multi-year project. Background Final Report to the Nova Scotia Habitat Conservation Fund: Determining the role of food availability on swallow population declines Project Supervisor: Tara Imlay, tara.imlay@dal.ca In the past

More information

Afring News. An electronic journal published by SAFRING, Animal Demography Unit at the University of Cape Town

Afring News. An electronic journal published by SAFRING, Animal Demography Unit at the University of Cape Town Afring News An electronic journal published by SAFRING, Animal Demography Unit at the University of Cape Town Afring News online accepts papers containing ringing information about birds. This includes

More information

Florida Field Naturalist

Florida Field Naturalist Florida Field Naturalist PUBLISHED BY THE FLORIDA ORNITHOLOGICAL SOCIETY VOL. 33, NO. 4 NOVEMBER 2005 PAGES 115-142 Florida Field Naturalist 33(4):115-122 2005. FLORIDA SCRUB-JAY EGG AND NESTLING PREDATION:

More information

Sparrowhawks & Goshawks and the Gymnogene

Sparrowhawks & Goshawks and the Gymnogene 1 Module # 6 Component # 7 Sparrowhawks & Goshawks and the Gymnogene Sparrowhawks and Goshawks There are nine Southern African species in this group, these are the: Ovambo Sparrowhawk Little Sparrowhawk

More information

Avian Ecology: Life History, Breeding Seasons, & Territories

Avian Ecology: Life History, Breeding Seasons, & Territories Avian Ecology: Life History, Breeding Seasons, & Territories Life History Theory Why do some birds lay 1-2 eggs whereas others 12+? Why do some species begin reproducing at < 1 year whereas others not

More information

THE BLUE PENGUIN (Eudyptula minor) AT TAIAROA HEAD, OTAGO,

THE BLUE PENGUIN (Eudyptula minor) AT TAIAROA HEAD, OTAGO, SCIENCE & RESEARCH SERIES NO.86 THE BLUE PENGUIN (Eudyptula minor) AT TAIAROA HEAD, OTAGO, 1992-1993 by Lyndon Perriman and Bruce McKinlay Published by Head Office, Department of Conservation, P 0 Box

More information

EverGraze: pastures to improve lamb weaning weights

EverGraze: pastures to improve lamb weaning weights EverGraze: pastures to improve lamb weaning weights S.M. Robertson and M.A. Friend EH Graham Centre for Agricultural Innovation, Charles Sturt University and NSW Department of Primary Industries, Wagga

More information

SEASONAL PATTERNS OF NESTING IN THE RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD MORTALITY

SEASONAL PATTERNS OF NESTING IN THE RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD MORTALITY Condor, 80:290-294 0 The Cooper Ornithological Society 1978 SEASONAL PATTERNS OF NESTING IN THE RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD MORTALITY DONALD F. CACCAMISE It is likely that birds adjust their reproductive period

More information

Activity 4 Building Bird Nests

Activity 4 Building Bird Nests Activity 4 Building Bird Nests Created By Point Reyes Bird Observatory Education Program Building Bird Nests Activity 4 Objective: To teach students about songbird nests, the different types, placement

More information

Anhinga anhinga (Anhinga or Snake-bird)

Anhinga anhinga (Anhinga or Snake-bird) Anhinga anhinga (Anhinga or Snake-bird) Family Anhingidae (Anhingas and Darters) Order: Pelecaniformes (Pelicans and Allied Waterbirds) Class: Aves (Birds) Fig. 1. Anhinga, Anhinga anhinga. [http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/accounts/anhinga_anhinga/,

More information

Analysis of Nest Record Cards for the Buzzard

Analysis of Nest Record Cards for the Buzzard Bird Study ISSN: 0006-3657 (Print) 1944-6705 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tbis20 Analysis of Nest Record Cards for the Buzzard C.R. Tubbs To cite this article: C.R. Tubbs (1972)

More information

Demography and breeding success of Falklands skua at Sea Lion Island, Falkland Islands

Demography and breeding success of Falklands skua at Sea Lion Island, Falkland Islands Filippo Galimberti and Simona Sanvito Elephant Seal Research Group Demography and breeding success of Falklands skua at Sea Lion Island, Falkland Islands Field work report - Update 2018/2019 25/03/2019

More information

He was a year older than her and experienced in how to bring up a brood and survive.

He was a year older than her and experienced in how to bring up a brood and survive. Great Tit 1. Life of a great tit 1.1. Courtship A young female great tit met her mate in a local flock in April. The male established a breeding territory and would sing, sway his head and display his

More information

Australian Journal of Zoology

Australian Journal of Zoology Publishing Australian Journal of Zoology Volume 49, 2001 CSIRO 2001 A journal for the publication of the results of original scientific research in all branches of zoology, except the taxonomy of invertebrates

More information

(199) THE HATCHING AND FLEDGING OF SOME COOT

(199) THE HATCHING AND FLEDGING OF SOME COOT (199) THE HATCHING AND FLEDGING OF SOME COOT BY RONALD ALLEY AND HUGH BOYD. SUCCESS INTRODUCTION. THE following data were obtained during the summer of 196, from observations carried out at Blagdon Reservoir,

More information

Multiple broods from a hole in the wall: breeding Red-and-yellow Barbets Trachyphonus erythrocephalus in southeast Sudan

Multiple broods from a hole in the wall: breeding Red-and-yellow Barbets Trachyphonus erythrocephalus in southeast Sudan Scopus 29: 11 15, December 2009 Multiple broods from a hole in the wall: breeding Red-and-yellow Barbets Trachyphonus erythrocephalus in southeast Sudan Marc de Bont Summary Nesting and breeding behaviour

More information

6 Month Progress Report. Cape vulture captive breeding and release programme Magaliesberg Mountains, South Africa. VulPro NPO

6 Month Progress Report. Cape vulture captive breeding and release programme Magaliesberg Mountains, South Africa. VulPro NPO 6 Month Progress Report Cape vulture captive breeding and release programme Magaliesberg Mountains, South Africa VulPro NPO Page Brooder and Incubator room construction 2 Cape Vulture captive bred chick

More information

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Clarke (1995) provides a comprehensive account.

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Clarke (1995) provides a comprehensive account. Circus aeruginosus 1. INTRODUCTION The marsh harrier (western marsh harrier) is increasing as a breeding species in Great Britain (Gibbons et al., 1993; Underhill-Day, 1998; Holling & RBBP, 2008) with

More information

2009 Eagle Nest News from Duke Farms eagle nest Written by Larissa Smith, Assistant Biologist

2009 Eagle Nest News from Duke Farms eagle nest Written by Larissa Smith, Assistant Biologist 2009 Eagle Nest News from Duke Farms eagle nest Written by Larissa Smith, Assistant Biologist July 7 - The youngest chick was gone from the nest this morning but has returned to the nest several times

More information

Group composition and reproductive success of cooperatively breeding white-winged choughs (Corcorax melanorhamphos) in urban and non-urban habitat

Group composition and reproductive success of cooperatively breeding white-winged choughs (Corcorax melanorhamphos) in urban and non-urban habitat Austral Ecology (2006) 31, 588 596 doi:10.1111/j.1442-9993.2006.01589.x Group composition and reproductive success of cooperatively breeding white-winged choughs (Corcorax melanorhamphos) in urban and

More information

THE BEGGING BEHAVIOR OF NESTLING EASTERN SCREECH-OWLS

THE BEGGING BEHAVIOR OF NESTLING EASTERN SCREECH-OWLS Wilson Bulletin, 110(l), 1998, pp. 86-92 THE BEGGING BEHAVIOR OF NESTLING EASTERN SCREECH-OWLS STEPHEN H. HOFSTETTER AND GARY RITCHISON J ABSTRACT-The behavior of adults and nestlings at nine Eastern Screech-owl

More information

Incubation feeding in snow buntings: female manipulation or indirect male parental care?

Incubation feeding in snow buntings: female manipulation or indirect male parental care? Behav Ecol Sociobiol (185) 17:27-284 Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Springer-Verlag 185 Incubation feeding in snow buntings: female manipulation or indirect male parental care? Bruce E. Lyon and Robert

More information

Effects of ewe age and season of lambing on proli cacy in US Targhee, Suffolk, and Polypay sheep

Effects of ewe age and season of lambing on proli cacy in US Targhee, Suffolk, and Polypay sheep Small Ruminant Research 38 (2000) 1±7 Effects of ewe age and season of lambing on proli cacy in US Targhee, Suffolk, and Polypay sheep D.R. Notter * Department of Animal and Poultry Sciences, Virginia

More information

The Heartfelt Story of our Backyard Bluebirds

The Heartfelt Story of our Backyard Bluebirds The Heartfelt Story of our Backyard Bluebirds My husband and I have had the privilege of being landlords to bluebirds for several years and we also monitor bluebird trails. We learn new things about these

More information

Do Tachycineta swallows use public information to choose nest sites?

Do Tachycineta swallows use public information to choose nest sites? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 Formatted for J Anim Ecol Jan 2012 Do Tachycineta swallows use public information to choose nest sites? not final author order: Dave Shutler 1*, André Desrochers

More information

Bald Eagles in the Yukon. Wildlife in our backyard

Bald Eagles in the Yukon. Wildlife in our backyard Bald Eagles in the Yukon Wildlife in our backyard The Bald Eagle at a glance Both male and female adult Bald Eagles have a dark brown body and wings with a white head, neck and tail. They have a yellow

More information

Demographic estimates from radio-tagging: models of age-speci c survival and breeding in the goshawk

Demographic estimates from radio-tagging: models of age-speci c survival and breeding in the goshawk Ecology 1999, Demographic estimates from radio-tagging: models of age-speci c survival and breeding in the goshawk R. E. KENWARD{, V. MARCSTROÈ M{ and M. KARLBOM{ {Institute of Terrestrial Furzebrook Research

More information

WHOO S WHOO? The Great Horned Owl as a Terrestrial Indicator Species in the Ecological Risk Assessment of the Tittabawassee River and Floodplain.

WHOO S WHOO? The Great Horned Owl as a Terrestrial Indicator Species in the Ecological Risk Assessment of the Tittabawassee River and Floodplain. WHOO S WHOO? The Great Horned Owl as a Terrestrial Indicator Species in the Ecological Risk Assessment of the Tittabawassee River and Floodplain. Chippewa Nature Center, April 27 2006 Sarah Coefield Doctoral

More information

Hatching asynchrony reduces the duration, not the magnitude, of peak load in breeding green-rumped parrotlets (Forpus passerinus)

Hatching asynchrony reduces the duration, not the magnitude, of peak load in breeding green-rumped parrotlets (Forpus passerinus) Behav Ecol Sociobiol (1999) 45: 444±450 Ó Springer-Verlag 1999 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Rodney B. Siegel á Wesley W. Weathers Steven R. Beissinger Hatching asynchrony reduces the duration, not the magnitude, of

More information

DISPERSAL IN THE COMMUNALLY BREEDING GROOVE-BILLED AN1 (CROTOPHAGA SULCIROSTRIS)

DISPERSAL IN THE COMMUNALLY BREEDING GROOVE-BILLED AN1 (CROTOPHAGA SULCIROSTRIS) The Condor 9152-64 0 The Cooper Ornithological Society I989 DISPERSAL IN THE COMMUNALLY BREEDING GROOVE-BILLED AN1 (CROTOPHAGA SULCIROSTRIS) BONNIE S. BOWEN,~ ROLF R. KOFORD~ AND SANDRA L. VEHRENCAMP Department

More information

BIOLOGY OF COOPERATIVE-BREEDING SCRUB JAYS (APHELOCOMA COERULESCENS) OF OAXACA, MEXICO

BIOLOGY OF COOPERATIVE-BREEDING SCRUB JAYS (APHELOCOMA COERULESCENS) OF OAXACA, MEXICO The Auk 110(2):207-214, 1993 BIOLOGY OF COOPERATIVE-BREEDING SCRUB JAYS (APHELOCOMA COERULESCENS) OF OAXACA, MEXICO D. BRENT BURT1'3 AND A. TOWNSEND PETERSON24 'Museum of Natural History, and Department

More information

Egg laying in the Blue Tit (Parus caeruleus):

Egg laying in the Blue Tit (Parus caeruleus): Chapter 2 Egg laying in the Blue Tit (Parus caeruleus): effect of temperature and interaction with food resource Fabrizio Grieco 24 Chapter 2 ABSTRACT Egg size and laying interruptions in a Blue Tit population

More information

PREDATION ON RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD EGGS AND NESTLINGS

PREDATION ON RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD EGGS AND NESTLINGS Wilson Bull., 91( 3), 1979, pp. 426-433 PREDATION ON RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD EGGS AND NESTLINGS FRANK S. SHIPLEY The contents of Red-winged Blackbird (Age&us phoeniceus) nests are subject to extensive and

More information

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Site occupation and territorial display Early April Mid-March to early May

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Site occupation and territorial display Early April Mid-March to early May Pandion haliaetus 1. INTRODUCTION The osprey (western osprey) is generally considered to have recolonised Scotland in 1954, after ceasing to breed about 1916 (Thom, 1986). Recently, however, it has been

More information

Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey

Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring - 2011 Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey October 2011 1 Cover photograph: Egyptian vulture landing in Beypazarı dump site, photographed

More information

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Chapman (1999) provides a comprehensive account.

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Chapman (1999) provides a comprehensive account. Falco subbuteo 1. INTRODUCTION The main breeding range of the hobby (Eurasian hobby) in Britain and Ireland lies in England, south of the Mersey/Humber line and extending into the borders of Wales. The

More information

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Caprimulgiformes Family: Podargidae Scientific Name: Podargus strigoides Common Name: Tawny frogmouth

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Caprimulgiformes Family: Podargidae Scientific Name: Podargus strigoides Common Name: Tawny frogmouth Order: Caprimulgiformes Family: Podargidae Scientific Name: Podargus strigoides Common Name: Tawny frogmouth AZA Management: Green Yellow Red None Photo (Male): Species is monomorphic Photo (Female): NATURAL

More information

Weaver Dunes, Minnesota

Weaver Dunes, Minnesota Hatchling Orientation During Dispersal from Nests Experimental analyses of an early life stage comparing orientation and dispersal patterns of hatchlings that emerge from nests close to and far from wetlands

More information

Everyday Mysteries: Why most male birds are more colorful than females

Everyday Mysteries: Why most male birds are more colorful than females Everyday Mysteries: Why most male birds are more colorful than females By Scientific American, adapted by Newsela staff on 02.06.17 Word Count 779 Mandarin ducks, a male (left) and a female, at WWT Martin

More information

EMBRYO DIAGNOSIS AN IMPORTANT TOOL TO HELP THE HATCHERY MANAGER

EMBRYO DIAGNOSIS AN IMPORTANT TOOL TO HELP THE HATCHERY MANAGER Issue No.14 / September 2007 EMBRYO DIAGNOSIS AN IMPORTANT TOOL TO HELP THE HATCHERY MANAGER By Avian Business Unit CEVA Santé Animale Libourne, France INTRODUCTION Chick quality is the first criterion

More information

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc 1. The money in the kingdom of Florin consists of bills with the value written on the front, and pictures of members of the royal family on the back. To test the hypothesis that all of the Florinese $5

More information

BREEDING SEASONALITY AND REPRODUCTIVE SUCCESS OF

BREEDING SEASONALITY AND REPRODUCTIVE SUCCESS OF BREEDING SEASONALITY AND REPRODUCTIVE SUCCESS OF WHITE-FRONTED BEE-EATERS IN KENYA PETER H. WREGE 1'2 AND STEPHEN T. EMLEN 1'2 Section of Neurobiology and Behavior, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York

More information